2003
DOI: 10.1007/s00359-003-0421-3
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visually guided orientation in flies: case studies in computational neuroethology

Abstract: To understand the functioning of nervous systems and, in particular, how they control behaviour we must bridge many levels of complexity from molecules, cells and synapses to perception behaviour. Although experimental analysis is a precondition for understanding by nervous systems, it is in no way sufficient. The understanding is aided at all levels of complexity by modelling. Modelling proved to be an inevitable tool to test the experimentally established hypotheses. In this review it will by exemplified by … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1
1

Citation Types

0
5
0

Year Published

2006
2006
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 8 publications
(5 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
0
5
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Once initiated, saccade dynamics are coordinated entirely by mechanosensory feedback [11], whereas flying straight and avoiding collisions require well-studied optomotor equilibrium reflexes [12]. Here, we show that saccades in the odor plume are fewer and smaller, but not altogether absent.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…Once initiated, saccade dynamics are coordinated entirely by mechanosensory feedback [11], whereas flying straight and avoiding collisions require well-studied optomotor equilibrium reflexes [12]. Here, we show that saccades in the odor plume are fewer and smaller, but not altogether absent.…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 65%
“…The evidence from that work points generally to cortical mechanisms for feature extraction. In contrast, a half century of work in flies has shown that these animals accomplish similar feature extraction within the secondary and tertiary optic ganglia—the medulla, lobula, and lobula plate (Egelhaaf, 1985a,b,c; Reichardt et al, 1989; Egelhaaf et al, 1993, 2003; Kimmerle and Egelhaaf, 2000; Aptekar et al, 2012; Fox et al, 2014). As these systems become more tractable with the advent of genetic tools for lesioning and imaging specific subsets of cells within these parts of the fly brain, in addition to the completion of full-fledged wiring diagrams, the need for more nuanced behavioral tools is acute.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, we here present some behavioural examples that strengthen the idea that all sensory processing must be seen as an ongoing and hence "active" process, where neither sensory images nor exploratory behaviours are fixed. Instead, animals make use of the changes in their sensory world that for instance occur whenever there is relative motion between fish and objects, something we here refer to as the electrical flow (Egelhaaf et al 2003;Karmeier et al 2006). In contrast to visual flow, however, electric flow is less suitable for orientation in complex electrical environments because electric images are superposition images (Migliaro et al 2005).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 96%