2021
DOI: 10.7554/elife.64278
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Visuomotor learning from postdictive motor error

Abstract: Sensorimotor learning adapts motor output to maintain movement accuracy. For saccadic eye movements, learning also alters space perception, suggesting a dissociation between the performed saccade and its internal representation derived from corollary discharge (CD). This is critical since learning is commonly believed to be driven by CD-based visual prediction error. We estimate the internal saccade representation through pre- and trans-saccadic target localization, showing that it decouples from the actual sa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
48
2

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

4
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 22 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 154 publications
(436 reference statements)
6
48
2
Order By: Relevance
“…The magnitude of the localization shift is the same for the inhibition and adaptation instruction, whereas secondary saccades occur less frequently following the inhibition instruction ( Heins et al, 2019 ). It thus appears most likely that the efference copy of the saccade is involved in the process of neural recalibration ( Bahcall & Kowler, 1999 , 2000 ; Cavanaugh, Berman, Joiner, & Wurtz, 2016 ; Collins & Wallman, 2012 ; Masselink & Lappe, 2021 ). In addition, our results indicate that the amplitude of the executed saccade does not always reflect this adaptation state, because the localization shift is equally strong for both instructions, whereas the adaptive changes to the saccade amplitude are not.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The magnitude of the localization shift is the same for the inhibition and adaptation instruction, whereas secondary saccades occur less frequently following the inhibition instruction ( Heins et al, 2019 ). It thus appears most likely that the efference copy of the saccade is involved in the process of neural recalibration ( Bahcall & Kowler, 1999 , 2000 ; Cavanaugh, Berman, Joiner, & Wurtz, 2016 ; Collins & Wallman, 2012 ; Masselink & Lappe, 2021 ). In addition, our results indicate that the amplitude of the executed saccade does not always reflect this adaptation state, because the localization shift is equally strong for both instructions, whereas the adaptive changes to the saccade amplitude are not.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…For instance, during development, incidental sensory consequences are the beginning of intended movement outcomes as infants learn the utility of their actions 44 . Similarly, intrinsic consequences such as pre-saccadic attention shifts 45 or efference copy signals (copies of motor commands that inform perceptual processing) 46 might adapt to anticipate intended and incidental consequences of movements 47 . We briefly introduce intrinsic and intended consequences before focusing on incidental consequences.…”
Section: Sensory Consequences Of Actionsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Instead, post-saccadic feedback is required to make any necessary adjustments to restore or maintain accuracy ( Collins & Wallman, 2012 ; Havermann & Lappe, 2010 ; Noto & Robinson, 2001 ; Wallman & Fuchs, 1998 ; Wong & Shelhamer, 2011 ). The adjustment of saccade amplitude, also termed saccade adaptation, is based on a comparison of the expected and actual sensory consequences of the eye movement, from which a motor error is postdicted ( Masselink & Lappe, 2021 ). Saccadic adaptation then aims to minimize this postdicted motor error.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%