2016
DOI: 10.1037/abn0000136
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Vocal expression in schizophrenia: Less than meets the ear.

Abstract: Abnormalities in nonverbal communication are a hallmark of schizophrenia. Results from studies using symptom rating scales suggest that these abnormalities are profound (i.e., 3–5 standard deviations) and occur across virtually every channel of vocal expression. Computerized acoustic analytic technologies, employed to overcome practical and psychometric limitations with symptom rating scales, have found much more benign and isolated abnormalities. In order to better understand vocal deficits in schizophrenia a… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

3
53
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
5
2
1

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 44 publications
(56 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
3
53
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Individuals with schizophrenia display atypical voice patterns, qualitatively described in terms of poverty of speech, increased pauses, distinctive tone and intensity of voice (Alpert et al, 2000;Andreasen et al, 1985;Cohen et al, 2016Cohen et al, , 2012bGalynker et al, 2000;Hoekert et al, 2007;Trémeau et al, 2005). Voice atypicalities have been reported since the first definitions of the disorder (Bleuler, 1911;Kraepelin, 1919), are used in the clinical assessment process, and assume an even stronger relevance in the light of growing findings associating voice patterns to cognitive function, emotional states, and social engagement Cohen and Hong, 2011;Faurholt-Jepsen et al, 2016;Nevler et al, 2017;Pisanski et al, 2016;Trigeorgis et al, 2016;Tsanas et al, 2011;Wang et al, 2015;Williams et al, 2014;Yin et al, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Individuals with schizophrenia display atypical voice patterns, qualitatively described in terms of poverty of speech, increased pauses, distinctive tone and intensity of voice (Alpert et al, 2000;Andreasen et al, 1985;Cohen et al, 2016Cohen et al, , 2012bGalynker et al, 2000;Hoekert et al, 2007;Trémeau et al, 2005). Voice atypicalities have been reported since the first definitions of the disorder (Bleuler, 1911;Kraepelin, 1919), are used in the clinical assessment process, and assume an even stronger relevance in the light of growing findings associating voice patterns to cognitive function, emotional states, and social engagement Cohen and Hong, 2011;Faurholt-Jepsen et al, 2016;Nevler et al, 2017;Pisanski et al, 2016;Trigeorgis et al, 2016;Tsanas et al, 2011;Wang et al, 2015;Williams et al, 2014;Yin et al, 2007).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This, in turn, may result in social withdrawal and the further aggravation of social cognitive impairments (Bambini et al, 2016;Bowie and Harvey, 2008;Del-Monte et al, 2013;Sparks et al, 2010;Tan et al, 2014;Thoma et al, 2009). Voice atypicalities may thus represent an important marker that parallels both clinical features and social cognitive functioning of individuals with schizophrenia over time (Cohen et al, 2016;Rapcan et al, 2010;Tahir et al, 2019).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…[23][24][25] Symbolic expression using "nonverbal" media involves many of the same neural substrates as verbal language (eg, music, 26 gestures, 27 and math 28 ), a notion formalized in theories about the origins of thought disorder in schizophrenia. 29 Moreover, clinical measures of thought disorder are often associated with measures of bizarre behavior (ie, nonverbal expression), 30,31 and measures of cognitive disorganization have been associated with increased creativity within the general population. 32 In sum, the RDoC language construct can be helpful for understanding thought disorder as an extreme variant of a potentially broad range of linguistic functions, which may help provide insight into its maladaptive and adaptive nature.…”
Section: How Can Rdoc Be Applied To the Study Of Thought Disorder?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Unfortunately, data from the CIDAR trial failed to find significant improvements in negative symptom severity or cognitive functioning with either galantamine or oxytocin compared to placebo using standard clinical rating scales or neuropsychological test batteries (Buchanan et al, 2016). The present study reanalyzed these data using computerized measures of natural speech and facial expressions as the dependent variables; measures with potentially superior reliability, sensitivity and precision than clinical rating scales for measuring blunted affect and alogia (Cohen and Elvevag, 2014; Cohen et al, 2016; Cohen et al, 2013). …”
Section: Dear Editormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Computerized assessment of Affect from Natural Speech protocol (Cohen et al, 2016) and FaceReader version 4.0, a commercially-available program developed by Noldus Information Technology (2010), were used to measure vocal and facial expressions respectively. Summary variables, selected based on recent studies (Cohen et al, 2016; Cohen et al, 2013), included: mean pause time (i.e., average voiceless epoch bounded by speech > 150ms in length), intonation (i.e., standard deviation of the fundamental frequency values computed within a voiced epoch [i.e., an “utterance”], then averaged across utterances), and emphasis (i.e., standard deviation of the volume computed within an utterance, then averaged across utterances), as were neutral , happy and negative (sum of sad, anger, scared) facial expressions . Extreme values were “winsorized” (i.e., replaced with values 3.5 SD from the overall means).…”
Section: Dear Editormentioning
confidence: 99%