2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Waiting for better, not for more: corvids respond to quality in two delay maintenance tasks

Abstract: Self-control, that is, overcoming impulsivity towards immediate gratification in favour of a greater but delayed reward, is seen as a valuable skill when making future-oriented decisions. Experimental studies in nonhuman primates revealed that individuals of some species are willing to tolerate delays of up to several minutes in order to gain food of a higher quantity or quality. Recently, birds (carrion crows, Corvus corone, common ravens, Corvus corax, Goffin cockatoos, Cacatua goffiniana) performed comparab… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

8
121
2
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
4
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 99 publications
(132 citation statements)
references
References 46 publications
8
121
2
1
Order By: Relevance
“…It is unlikely that the different results obtained on capuchins in the present study and on corvids and cockatoos (Auersperg et al 2013;Hillemann et al 2014;Wascher et al 2012) are due to species differences in sensitivity to quantity and quality of food rewards. In fact, also capuchin monkeys performed better in the qualitative version of a delayed exchange task than in its quantitative version (Drapier et al 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 48%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…It is unlikely that the different results obtained on capuchins in the present study and on corvids and cockatoos (Auersperg et al 2013;Hillemann et al 2014;Wascher et al 2012) are due to species differences in sensitivity to quantity and quality of food rewards. In fact, also capuchin monkeys performed better in the qualitative version of a delayed exchange task than in its quantitative version (Drapier et al 2005).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 48%
“…the delayed exchange task and the accumulation task) carried out in birds. Corvids (Corvus corax, Corvus corone corone) and Goffin's cockatoos (Cacatua goffiniana) tested in a delayed exchange task, in which they were required to return an initial food item after a certain delay in order to obtain a higher-value reward, performed well only when they could exchange a low-quality food item for a high-quality food item, whereas they could not wait when they were required to exchange a smaller food amount for a larger amount of the same food (Auersperg et al 2013;Hillemann et al 2014;Wascher et al 2012). These same species tested in the accumulation task (in which four food items were accumulated by the experimenter at a fixed rate within reach of the subject as long as it refrained from taking the food) performed better with foods of different quality rather than with different quantities of the same food (Hillemann et al 2014).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Three of four chimpanzees showed this pattern, reflective of self-distraction-a form of cognitive control that allowed them to deal with an apparent sense of their own fallibility in this task. Whether other species, including other primates, might employ such self-distraction techniques is not known, although there is some suggestive evidence that a grey parrot engaged in self-distraction strategies during a delay of gratification test (Koepke, Gray, & Pepperberg, 2015), and other bird species can succeed in delay of gratification tasks (e.g., Auersperg, Laumer, & Bugnyar, 2013;Hillemann, Bugnyar, Kotrschal, & Wascher, 2014).…”
Section: Dealing With Fallibility: Strategic Delay Of Gratificationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Furthermore, because food motivation is 119 known to influence problem-solving behaviour (Laland & Reader, 1999; Sol, Griffin & Bartomeus, 120 2012;Griffin, Diquelou & Perea, 2014;Griffin & Guez, 2014), we tested subjects early in the 121 5 morning without feeding them the evening prior to the test. Finally, as food motivation is influenced 122 by food quality (Fontenot et al, 2007;Dufour et al, 2012;Hillemann et al, 2014); we used high 123 value food (based on a previously performed preference test) for testing (Rao, et al in press). 124…”
Section: Introduction 23mentioning
confidence: 99%