2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.014
|View full text |Cite|
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What infants know about the unsaid: Phonological categorization in the absence of auditory input

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
4
1

Relationship

0
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 5 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…LOOK words may show the highest recall, as silent looking may be the least cognitively demanding task for younger children (LOOK > LISTEN > SAY; distinctiveness and cognitive demand hypothesis: Possibility 1). On the other hand, LOOK words may require more cognitive resources for younger children: When an image is provided without an auditory label, children must independently retrieve the word's phonological representation (Ngon & Peperkamp, 2016). This retrieval needed in the LOOK condition may turn out to be more demanding for young children compared to the LISTEN condition, where the auditory label is provided with the image (LISTEN > LOOK & SAY; distinctiveness and cognitive demand hypothesis: Possibility 2).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…LOOK words may show the highest recall, as silent looking may be the least cognitively demanding task for younger children (LOOK > LISTEN > SAY; distinctiveness and cognitive demand hypothesis: Possibility 1). On the other hand, LOOK words may require more cognitive resources for younger children: When an image is provided without an auditory label, children must independently retrieve the word's phonological representation (Ngon & Peperkamp, 2016). This retrieval needed in the LOOK condition may turn out to be more demanding for young children compared to the LISTEN condition, where the auditory label is provided with the image (LISTEN > LOOK & SAY; distinctiveness and cognitive demand hypothesis: Possibility 2).…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Research on developmental speech production takes a somewhat indirect approach to examine the role or effect of production on learning—in other words, production is typically not a controlled factor. Evidence is emerging that production‐based representations exist even before infants produce meaningful speech (Bruderer, Danielson, Kandhadai, & Werker, ; DePaolis, Vihman, & Keren‐Portnoy, ; DePaolis, Vihman, & Nakai, ; Majorano, Vihman, & DePaolis, ; Ngon & Peperkamp, ; Yeung & Werker, ). In addition to observing that there is a relationship between infants’ babbling repertories and their first words (Vihman, Macken, Miller, Simmons & Miller, ), children are more likely to add words to their productive vocabulary when the words are shorter in word length, have more phonological neighbours (i.e., words that sound similar to many other words), and are more frequent (Carlson, Sonderegger, & Bane, ; Coady & Aslin, ; Maekawa & Storkel, ; Ota & Green, ; Stoel‐Gammon, ; Storkel, , ).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, more recent research has found that infants can name objects implicitly, and that private speech may not be dependent on expressive ability (Mani & Plunkett, ). For instance, it has been shown that 21‐month‐olds can activate phonological representations of words they understand but have not yet attempted to pronounce (Ngon & Peperkamp, ). As such, our results are in favor of receptive rather than expressive language as a predictor of both hot and cool aspects of self‐regulation, but that this does not rule out the involvement of private speech.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%