1992
DOI: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02453.x
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

What might have been: Counterfactual thought concerning personal decisions

Abstract: Counterfactual thinking entails the process of imagining alternatives to realitywhat might have been. The present study explores the incidence and content of counterfactual thinking about personal decisions in three samples of adults. The results indicate, first, that counterfactual thought occurs frequently among normal adults, with approximately half of each sample reporting that they would do something differently if they had their lives to live over. Secondly, there appear to be common themes to the counte… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
45
0

Year Published

1995
1995
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 49 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 7 publications
7
45
0
Order By: Relevance
“…1 The sample consisted of adults of varying ages and occupations approached by graduate counseling students as part of a class project. Landman and Manis (1992) used the same measurement strategy in the following three samples: undergraduates, adult women who had contacted the University of Michigan Women's Center, and a collection of adults culled from a motor vehicle licensing database. DeGenova (1992) asked a representative sample of elderly residents of Lafayette, Indiana, what aspects of their lives they would change if they could; respondents gave ratings of desired change for 35 life domains.…”
Section: Study 1: Meta-analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…1 The sample consisted of adults of varying ages and occupations approached by graduate counseling students as part of a class project. Landman and Manis (1992) used the same measurement strategy in the following three samples: undergraduates, adult women who had contacted the University of Michigan Women's Center, and a collection of adults culled from a motor vehicle licensing database. DeGenova (1992) asked a representative sample of elderly residents of Lafayette, Indiana, what aspects of their lives they would change if they could; respondents gave ratings of desired change for 35 life domains.…”
Section: Study 1: Meta-analysismentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The Hattiangadi et al (1995) data for example show that inactions outnumbered actions by more than four to one, and that does not include a category of "indeterminate regrets" (those coded as "both" or "neither") which in the studies reported in the thesis were on average three times more likely to be general than specific. It would not be surprising to find similar patterns in surveys asking people to say what they would do if they had their lives to live over (DeGenoa, 1992;Hattiangadi et al, 1995;Kinnier & Metha, 1989;Landman & Manis, 1992;Landman et al, 1995) because as Kahneman (1995) has suggested, requests for regrets tend to elicit elaborative counterfactuals about how life might have been better, which are likely to involve big changes of a general nature. In temporal construal terms (Trope & Liberman, 2003) such requests might be interpreted as an invitation to consider personal goals at the superordinate level, which would necessarily elicit "bigger picture" construals of distant events.…”
Section: Factors That Make General Inactionsmentioning
confidence: 84%
“…Men and women think regret-related counterfactual thoughts with equal frequency (Landman & Manis, 1992), although sex differences have been found: women are more likely than men to report family and relationship regrets (Jokisaari, 2004) and within the domain of romantic relationships men are more likely than women to regret not having had more partners (Roese, Pennington, Janicki, Li & Kenrick, 2006). Regret is found in samples of young and old participants (Wrosch & Heckhausen 2002), in cross-cultural studies of American, Chinese, Japanese and Russian samples (Gilovich, Wang, Regan & Nishina, 2003) and in studies carried out independently in Turkey (Toktas, 2002),…”
Section: Who Experiences Regret What Do People Regret and Why?mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Likewise, economists do make extensive use of counterfactual reasoning, mostly self-unconsciously though. For example, a vast literature is concerned with economic and financial decisions: If it is true that, normatively, von Neumann and Morgenstern's (1944) utility theory maintains its charm among decision theorists and economists, in a descriptive approach the role of counterfactuals in decision-making processes is highly recognized in regret theory (Bell, 1982(Bell, , 1985Sugden, 1982, 1986), which is gaining ground among cognitive psychologists (Zeelenberg et al, 1996;Zeelenberg and Beattie 1997;Tsiros, 1998); implications for marketing are also studied (McGill, 2000;Roese, 2000); further, the way counterfactuals affect economic and financial decisions is investigated (Lundberg and Frost, 1992;McConnell et al, 2000;Tsiros and Mittal, 2000), as well as their impact on a personal domain (Landman and Manis, 1992). Nevertheless, so far as I am aware, the role of counterfactual thought in the formulation of economic concepts has not yet been adequately recognized, let alone the epistemological implications on economic models (but see Sugden, 2000, for an interpretation of economic models as counterfactual worlds; see also Elster, 1978) and, in general, on decisionmaking processes.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%