Over a century of psychological research provides strong and consistent support for the idea that cognitive ability correlates positively with success in tasks that people face in employment, education, and everyday life. Recent experimental research, however, has converged on a different and provocative conclusion, namely that lower-ability people can actually be more effective performers within special environments characterized by features such as time pressure, social evaluation and unpredictable task change. If this conclusion is true, it has extensive implications for practices such as personnel selection, training design, and teaching at all levels.The current paper re-examines and reinterprets this research within the context of wellestablished resource theories of cognitive processing and skill acquisition leading to a less provocative conclusion that serves to reiterate the benefits of cognitive ability for task performance. Following this re-examination, we conclude by providing a research agenda for examining the determinants of skilled performance in dynamic task environments, including: (a) broadening the range of abilities and task difficulties examined, (b) considering the role of nonability traits and goals in skilled performance (e.g., personality, learning and performance goals), (c) investigating the processes (e.g., problem solving strategies) that people use in complex environments, (d) developing research designs and analytic strategies for examining adaptive performance, and (e) investigating how best to train for adaptive performance. Over a century of psychological research has provided a wealth of empirical support for cognitive ability as one of the most critical determinants of skilled performance. Setting aside the nature-nurture controversies surrounding the development of peoples' cognitive ability (Neisser, 1998), it is a well established and uncontroversial finding that cognitive ability can be reliably measured and is a consistent and strong predictor of important societal outcomes, such as work performance and career accomplishments (Campbell, McCloy, Oppler, & Sager, 1993;Hunter & Hunter, 1984;Neisser, et al., 1996; Park, Lubinski, & Benbow, 2007;Schmidt & Hunter, 2004), performance in academic environments (Binet & Simon, 1916;Kuncel, Hezlett, & Ones, 2001), and successful navigation of the complexities of everyday life (Gottfredson, 1997;Griener, Snowdon, & Schmitt, 1996). Certainly, theoretical disagreements about the exact nature and structure of cognitive abilities remain (e.g., see the exchange by Ackerman, Beier, & Boyle, 2005;Kane, Hambrick, & Conway, 2005;Oberauer, Schulze, Wilhelm, & Süß, 2005), and nonability traits such as interests, personality, and motivation are critically important determinants of performance (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). Nonetheless, there is a strong scientific consensus that higher levels of cognitive ability yield positive benefits for virtually all types of skilled performance.A spate of recent research, however, has directly chall...