2020
DOI: 10.1002/jcpy.1175
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When and Why Choices for Others Diverge from Consumers' Own Salient Goals

Abstract: Consumers frequently make choices and purchase products for other people (e.g., buying a gift for a friend). While extant research identified many factors that influence how choices for others are made, much of this literature focused on product-specific factors or motivations pertaining to the process of exchange to understand choice-for-others phenomena. Little is known about the influence of consumer-relevant factors on choices made for other people. In the current research, we examine how choices for other… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
10
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
8

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 12 publications
(11 citation statements)
references
References 62 publications
(139 reference statements)
1
10
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Previous studies have noted the positive effects of salient goals (e.g. intentional and activated eco-behaviour goals) on consumer choices, inclinations, decisions and actions (Bryksina, 2020;Becker et al, 2020), supporting the logic of goal-behaviour congruence (Bryksina, 2020;Emekci, 2019). Hence, we posit in this study that:…”
Section: Eco-behaviour Goalseco-brand Orientationsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…Previous studies have noted the positive effects of salient goals (e.g. intentional and activated eco-behaviour goals) on consumer choices, inclinations, decisions and actions (Bryksina, 2020;Becker et al, 2020), supporting the logic of goal-behaviour congruence (Bryksina, 2020;Emekci, 2019). Hence, we posit in this study that:…”
Section: Eco-behaviour Goalseco-brand Orientationsupporting
confidence: 81%
“…One common manner by which modifications to scale dimensionality occurs is via the use of a dimensional subset of an original scale, typically one or a few dimensions from a multidimensional measure (this was the most common form of scope change in our JCP review and occurred most frequently with validated scale usage). For instance, from our JCP review Farmer et al (2021) use the intolerance of ambiguity dimension of Webster and Kruglanski's (1994) need for a cognitive closure scale as their mediator in assessing how political ideology affects the response to ambiguity, whereas three different articles (Bryksina, 2020; Granulo et al, 2021; Li et al, 2021) only use the 11‐item counter‐conformity subscale of the need for uniqueness scale (Tian et al, 2001). In these cases, the authors argue reasonably that a particular subdimension was most relevant to their conceptualization, resulting in no assessment of the other dimensions.…”
Section: Considerations For “As Is” and Modified Scale Deploymentmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Here we show that actions that facilitate the progress of another person (i.e., a group member) are more motivating in pursuit of a group-level goal than actions that facilitate one's own progress. This differs from what occurs in individual-level goals, where actions that facilitate one's own progress are motivating (Bryksina, 2020;Etkin & Laran, 2019;Kopetz et al, 2012;van Osselaer et al, 2005;van Osselaer & Janiszewski, 2012), and actions that facilitate the progress of another person can be demotivating (Bryksina, 2020). The second contribution of this research is to the literature on perceived goal progress as the driver of motivation.…”
mentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Motivation-a process that explains peoples' persistence, intensity, and direction of goal-consistent actions (Geen, 1995;Larson, 2010)can be influenced by several factors. In general, individuals are motivated to undertake actions that generate the most impact/progress toward their salient goal (Bryksina, 2020;Etkin & Laran, 2019;Kopetz et al, 2012;van Osselaer et al, 2005;van Osselaer & Janiszewski, 2012), and this holds for both individual-and grouplevel goals (e.g., Karau & Williams, 1993;Kerr, 1996). For example, people are more motivated to give to charity if they think that their actions are making greater impact/progress toward the charitable goal (Cryder et al, 2013).…”
Section: Determinants Of Motivation In Groupsmentioning
confidence: 99%