2008
DOI: 10.3758/pbr.15.5.997
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When confidence in a choice is independent of which choice is made

Abstract: ), participants chose an answer to two-alternative general-information questions and indicated their confidence in that answer. Whereas previous studies focused on the determinants of the choice and, consequently, on the confidence in that choice, the present study examined a component of subjective confidence that correlates with properties of the items itself, no matter which answer is chosen.The item-based approach to the study of metacognitive judgments has proved useful in the past. According to this appr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
38
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
7
2
1

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 30 publications
(39 citation statements)
references
References 30 publications
1
38
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Underlying much of the work in metacognition is the Monitoring → Control (MC) model, which assumes that metacognitive judgments (monitoring) drive and guide the strategic regulation of information processing and behaviour (control; Barnes, Nelson, Dunlosky, Mazzoni & Narens, 1999; Nelson & Narens, 1990; Son & Schwartz, 2002). Consistent with this model are findings suggesting that JOLs during self‐paced learning affect the allocation of study time (Dunlosky & Hertzog, 1998; Lockl & Schneider, 2003; Nelson & Leonesio, 1988), FOK judgments determine the amount of time spent searching for an elusive memory target before giving up, and confidence judgments in a belief or attitude determine whether people translate that belief or attitude into behaviour (Fischhoff, Slovic & Lichtenstein, 1977; Gill, Swann & Silvera, 1998; Goldsmith & Koriat, 2008; Koriat, 2008c). These findings suggest that metacognitive judgments are not mere epiphenomena but actually influence behaviour.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Underlying much of the work in metacognition is the Monitoring → Control (MC) model, which assumes that metacognitive judgments (monitoring) drive and guide the strategic regulation of information processing and behaviour (control; Barnes, Nelson, Dunlosky, Mazzoni & Narens, 1999; Nelson & Narens, 1990; Son & Schwartz, 2002). Consistent with this model are findings suggesting that JOLs during self‐paced learning affect the allocation of study time (Dunlosky & Hertzog, 1998; Lockl & Schneider, 2003; Nelson & Leonesio, 1988), FOK judgments determine the amount of time spent searching for an elusive memory target before giving up, and confidence judgments in a belief or attitude determine whether people translate that belief or attitude into behaviour (Fischhoff, Slovic & Lichtenstein, 1977; Gill, Swann & Silvera, 1998; Goldsmith & Koriat, 2008; Koriat, 2008c). These findings suggest that metacognitive judgments are not mere epiphenomena but actually influence behaviour.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…For example, recent work using general information questions showed that semantic cue familiarity (i.e., familiarity with the overall topic of the general information question) was associated with higher confidence not only for correct answers, but also for incorrect answers (e.g., a person who knew a lot about computers would be more confident on questions about computers(Koriat, 2008). In such paradigms, confidence judgments are based on semantic familiarity with the cue because participants rest on the belief that more knowledge about that topic means they are more accurate, yet this may be an irrelevant factor.…”
Section: Methodsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Dissociations between confidence and accuracy in experimental studies suggest the role of other factors in determining the confidence level, besides strength of the memory trace [24]- [25]. In an experimental task where subjects chose an answer to a question with two alternatives, Koriat [26] found that a higher level of familiarity about the question's domain can increase the subject's confidence level, independent of response accuracy. Based on the hypothesis that greater familiarity causes a higher level of confidence, we assumed that a meta-level recognition process computes the recognition point by monitoring (1) the WTA connectionist network used in finding the IP, as well as (2) the Familiarity Unit for measuring confidence.…”
Section: E Summary Ofresultsmentioning
confidence: 96%