2016
DOI: 10.3758/s13421-016-0680-1
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When fast logic meets slow belief: Evidence for a parallel-processing model of belief bias

Abstract: Two experiments pitted the default-interventionist account of belief bias against a parallel-processing model. According to the former, belief bias occurs because a fast, belief-based evaluation of the conclusion pre-empts a working-memory demanding logical analysis. In contrast, according to the latter both belief-based and logic-based responding occur in parallel. Participants were given deductive reasoning problems of variable complexity and instructed to decide whether the conclusion was valid on half the … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

12
92
0
11

Year Published

2017
2017
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
4
2
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 87 publications
(115 citation statements)
references
References 64 publications
12
92
0
11
Order By: Relevance
“…First, although our behavioral results replicate the classic differential performance across modus ponens and modus tollens type inferences, accuracy in the modus tollens type inferences was higher than typically reported in classic studies (i.e., 87% in our work vs. 63% in Taplin, 1971; 62% in Wildman & Fletcher, 1977). Nonetheless, our accuracy rates are in line with a number of behavioral and neuroimaging reports (e.g., 94% in Prado et al , 84% in Luo, Yang, Du, & Zhang, ; between 80% and 88% in Bloomfield & Rips ; 79% in Knauff et al, ; 78% in Trippas, Thompson, & Handley, ; 75% in Evans, 1977; and above 90% in the Wason Selection Task as implemented in Li, Zhang, Luo, Qiu, & Liu, , Qiu et al, , Liu et al, ) as well as developmental work showing that by age 16 accuracy rates for modus tollens range between 78% and 87% (Daniel et al, 2006). We do stress, however, that although our participants did not undergo any overt training (e.g., training to criterion; see for example Reverberi et al, ) and reported no formal training in logic, our procedure selected high‐performance individuals in the sense that they had to meet a 60% accuracy criterion across each of the four conditions (complex/simple, deductive/non‐deductive).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…First, although our behavioral results replicate the classic differential performance across modus ponens and modus tollens type inferences, accuracy in the modus tollens type inferences was higher than typically reported in classic studies (i.e., 87% in our work vs. 63% in Taplin, 1971; 62% in Wildman & Fletcher, 1977). Nonetheless, our accuracy rates are in line with a number of behavioral and neuroimaging reports (e.g., 94% in Prado et al , 84% in Luo, Yang, Du, & Zhang, ; between 80% and 88% in Bloomfield & Rips ; 79% in Knauff et al, ; 78% in Trippas, Thompson, & Handley, ; 75% in Evans, 1977; and above 90% in the Wason Selection Task as implemented in Li, Zhang, Luo, Qiu, & Liu, , Qiu et al, , Liu et al, ) as well as developmental work showing that by age 16 accuracy rates for modus tollens range between 78% and 87% (Daniel et al, 2006). We do stress, however, that although our participants did not undergo any overt training (e.g., training to criterion; see for example Reverberi et al, ) and reported no formal training in logic, our procedure selected high‐performance individuals in the sense that they had to meet a 60% accuracy criterion across each of the four conditions (complex/simple, deductive/non‐deductive).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 90%
“…While there is empirical support for the additional cognitive challenge that negation provides (Schaeken & Schroyens, 2000; Schroyens et al, 2000, 2001), it might not be strong enough for us to observe differences in conflict detection. This possibility would be congruent with the results of Trippas et al (2017), who indicated in their supplementary material that they found no significant difference in conflict detection between the propositional versions of MP and MT, but still reported smaller conflict detection effects for syllogisms of greater complexity than MT. Combined with these results, our study suggests that MT might be simple enough for people to develop an intuitive grasp of its structure.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 89%
“…Note that, while conflict detection on propositional versions of MP and MT has been previously observed (Trippas et al, 2017), to our knowledge, our studies are the first to manipulate complexity with these categorical items and with these four inferences forms.…”
Section: Introductionsupporting
confidence: 49%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…However, note that other options are available, including the use of a dynamic discrete-state model approach (e.g., Klauer, 2018). Although response times have not played a significant role in this literature, they nevertheless introduce important theoretical constraints (e.g., Trippas et al, 2017). This state of affairs is partly due to the difficulties associated with fitting such models when individual data are sparse.…”
Section: Playing a More Ambitious Gamementioning
confidence: 99%