2009
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.005
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When some is actually all: Scalar inferences in face-threatening contexts

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

12
105
3

Year Published

2014
2014
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
3
3
1

Relationship

1
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 157 publications
(120 citation statements)
references
References 16 publications
12
105
3
Order By: Relevance
“…We conducted an analysis of variance in which the predictors were the valence of the target statement and the presence or absence of Well. This analysis detected a main effect of valence (consistent with Bonnefon et al, 2009) In other words, the presence of Well encouraged scalar inferences from positive statements ("some people loved your idea"), without discouraging scalar inferences from negative statements ("some people hated your idea").…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 52%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We conducted an analysis of variance in which the predictors were the valence of the target statement and the presence or absence of Well. This analysis detected a main effect of valence (consistent with Bonnefon et al, 2009) In other words, the presence of Well encouraged scalar inferences from positive statements ("some people loved your idea"), without discouraging scalar inferences from negative statements ("some people hated your idea").…”
Section: Methodssupporting
confidence: 52%
“…It has been repeatedly demonstrated that scalar terms are less likely to generate a scalar inference when they contain a face-threatening content such as a criticism, an imposition, or a piece of bad news (Bonnefon, Feeney, & Villejoubert, 2009;Bonnefon & Villejoubert, 2006;Feeney & Bonnefon, 2013;Juanchich & Sirota, 2013;Juanchich, Sirota, & Butler, 2012;Pighin & Bonnefon, 2011;. Consider the following examples:…”
mentioning
confidence: 97%
“…There is now considerable evidence that scalar implicature machinery is deployed flexibly: Listeners are less likely to make scalar implicatures when they believe that the speaker is not motivated to be informative (Bonnefon, Feeney, & Villejoubert, 2009) or is unlikely to know whether the more informative statement is true (Bergen & Grodner, 2012). However, the machinery may be deployed flexibly but still operate over a lexical representation rather than over the proposition as a whole.…”
Section: Informativity: Lexical or Higher-level?mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Second, despite an explosion of behavioral experiments on scalar implicature (Barner, Brooks, & Bale, 2010; Bonnefon et al, 2009; Bott & Noveck, 2004; Breheny, Ferguson, & Katsos, 2012; Chemla & Spector, 2011; Chevallier et al, 2008; Chevallier, Wilson, Happé, & Noveck, 2010; De Neys & Schaeken, 2007; Feeney, Scrafton, Duckworth, & Handley, 2004; Goodman & Stuhlmuller, 2013; Foppolo, Guasti, & Chierchia, 2012; Grodner et al, 2010; Huang & Snedeker, 2009a, 2009b, 2011; Marty, Chemla & Spector, 2013; Noveck, 2001; Papafragou & Musolino, 2003; Papafragou & Tantalou, 2004; Papafragou, 2006; Pouscoulous, Noveck, Politzer, & Bastide, 2007) relatively little is known about the neural computation of scalar implicature. Two studies have investigated the brain response produced when a scalar implicature conflicts with world knowledge.…”
Section: Overview Of the Experimentsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation