2014
DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

When stereotypes meet robots: The double-edge sword of robot gender and personality in human–robot interaction

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

9
228
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
6
1

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 340 publications
(238 citation statements)
references
References 49 publications
9
228
1
Order By: Relevance
“…A potential explanation might be that the advisor's task was not associated with any explicit gender stereotypes, so participants held no expectations as to whether a male or female advisor should know the alien's taste better. Earlier research has indeed shown that gender stereotyping of tasks is manifested in the interaction among real humans (see Jeanquart-Barone and Sekaran, 1994;Eagly, 1997) and also in the interaction between a human and a robotic partner (see Kuchenbrandt et al, 2014;Tay et al, 2014). Thus, future research could investigate whether gender similarity (between user and robot) influences trusting beliefs and behavior when the task the robot has to perform is clearly related to gender stereotypes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…A potential explanation might be that the advisor's task was not associated with any explicit gender stereotypes, so participants held no expectations as to whether a male or female advisor should know the alien's taste better. Earlier research has indeed shown that gender stereotyping of tasks is manifested in the interaction among real humans (see Jeanquart-Barone and Sekaran, 1994;Eagly, 1997) and also in the interaction between a human and a robotic partner (see Kuchenbrandt et al, 2014;Tay et al, 2014). Thus, future research could investigate whether gender similarity (between user and robot) influences trusting beliefs and behavior when the task the robot has to perform is clearly related to gender stereotypes.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Another three Likert scales of 7 levels inquired agreement with statements that were adapted from the perceived trust questionnaires (Heerink et al, 2009;Tay et al, 2014) including: I will trust the advisor if the advisor gives me advice again in the future, I trust that the advisor can provide me correct answers to the game, and I will follow the advice that the advisor gives me. (ii) Nine semantic differential items with seven levels adapted from the individualized trust scale questionnaire (Wheeless and Grotz, 1977) with the following poles: untrustworthy-trustworthy, unreliable-reliable, insincere-sincere, dishonest-honest, distrustful-trustful, inconsiderate-considerate, divulging-confidential, deceitful-not deceitful, and disrespectful-respectful.…”
Section: Measure Trusting Beliefsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The matching of robot's physical embodiment, perceived gender entity and gendered based role within humans' gender expectations is thought to improve HRI and elicit attitudes that are more positive. For example, Tay, Jung, and Park () found that participants rated robots more positively that matched stereotypical gender‐occupational role and personality‐occupational role. However, gender assignment relies heavily on roboticists' common‐sense assumptions about female and male gender roles (Robertson, ), contributing to the maintenance of gender‐based stereotypes.…”
Section: Drawbacks To the Humanization Of Robotsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Moreover, recent research has demonstrated that social robots are more easily accepted when they conform with stereotypes that match their occupational role (e.g. healthcare, security) to the "gender" and "personality" that they are designed to possess [15].…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%