2008
DOI: 10.1353/ol.0.0017
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Whence the Austronesian Indirect Possession Construction?

Abstract: Possession in some Austronesian languages shows levels of elaboration far in excess of cross-linguistic norms, while in others it is strikingly unelaborated. The appearance of alienable/inalienable contrasts has been assumed to result from contact with Papuan languages, and the existence of a paradigm of indirect possessive classifiers is cited as one of the pieces of evidence for the Oceanic subgroup, while acknowledging that indirect possession constructions can be found in Malayo-Polynesian languages furthe… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

0
7
0

Year Published

2009
2009
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 20 publications
(7 citation statements)
references
References 17 publications
0
7
0
Order By: Relevance
“…They have been variously affiliated with the widespread Trans New Guinea languages that are said to span New Guinea from east to west (Stokhof 1975;Voorhoeve 1975), and to the West Papuan languages of the Bird's Head in north-west New Guinea (Capell 1975;Donohue 2008). There is a mainland base for the term among Papuan languages on the north-western fringe of New Guinea, and then a spread south and west, following the area in which we see a Papuan substrate in the grammar of the languages (Capell 1975;Donohue and Grimes 2008;Donohue and Schapper 2008). The area in which *muku is found in the Austronesian languages of Maluku connects the New Guinea beachhead to the Timor area, where Papuan languages are still spoken today.…”
Section: The Distribution Of Banana Termsmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…They have been variously affiliated with the widespread Trans New Guinea languages that are said to span New Guinea from east to west (Stokhof 1975;Voorhoeve 1975), and to the West Papuan languages of the Bird's Head in north-west New Guinea (Capell 1975;Donohue 2008). There is a mainland base for the term among Papuan languages on the north-western fringe of New Guinea, and then a spread south and west, following the area in which we see a Papuan substrate in the grammar of the languages (Capell 1975;Donohue and Grimes 2008;Donohue and Schapper 2008). The area in which *muku is found in the Austronesian languages of Maluku connects the New Guinea beachhead to the Timor area, where Papuan languages are still spoken today.…”
Section: The Distribution Of Banana Termsmentioning
confidence: 92%
“…As in some other Papuan languages (Kalam, see Blevins & Pawley 2010, and Kanum, see Donohue 2009), the nuclei of many syllables in Nen are epenthetic schwas, e.g. [ə.ə] for / / ‘come!’.…”
Section: Vowelsmentioning
confidence: 89%
“…In one paper already discussed, Xu et al (2012: 4574) assert that, prior to Austronesian dispersal, ISEA consisted of 'Mesolithic hunter-gatherer communities, ' implying that these communities did not have a significant impact on the societies that subsequently emerged. Tere is ample evidence from substratal signals in the Austronesian languages of ISEA that contact with the pre-Austronesian populations of the region was highly significant in shaping the modern languages (e.g., Reid, 1994;Donohue, 2004Donohue, , 2005Donohue, , 2007aDonohue, , 2007bDonohue and Schapper, 2008;Klamer et al, 2008, and many others). Tis suggests that, rather than being overlooked without consequence, the indigenous ISEA populations were socially prominent enough in post-Austronesian ISEA to continue, over a long period, to affect the lexicon, phonology, morphology and syntax of the new Austronesian languages.…”
Section: Isea Was Not a 'No-[hu]man's Land'mentioning
confidence: 99%