2015
DOI: 10.1111/bjop.12161
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Who can escape the natural number bias in rational number tasks? A study involving students and experts

Abstract: Many learners have difficulties with rational number tasks because they persistently rely on their natural number knowledge, which is not always applicable. Studies show that such a natural number bias can mislead not only children but also educated adults. It is still unclear whether and under what conditions mathematical expertise enables people to be completely unaffected by such a bias on tasks in which people with less expertise are clearly biased. We compared the performance of eighth-grade students and … Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

6
11
0
2

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
8
2

Relationship

1
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 29 publications
(19 citation statements)
references
References 44 publications
6
11
0
2
Order By: Relevance
“…It is particularly in line with research on academic mathematicians who solved almost all fraction comparison problems correctly but showed a natural number bias in terms of response times in problems with common components (Obersteiner et al, 2013). It seems that the natural number bias is particularly likely to occur in problems in which participants focus strongly on the natural number components (Alibali & Sidney, 2015;Obersteiner, Van Hoof, Verschaffel, & Van Dooren, 2016). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…It is particularly in line with research on academic mathematicians who solved almost all fraction comparison problems correctly but showed a natural number bias in terms of response times in problems with common components (Obersteiner et al, 2013). It seems that the natural number bias is particularly likely to occur in problems in which participants focus strongly on the natural number components (Alibali & Sidney, 2015;Obersteiner, Van Hoof, Verschaffel, & Van Dooren, 2016). …”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…These response patterns align with dual process theories that rely on the distinction and interaction between intuitive and analytical reasoning processes (Gillard, Van Dooren, Schaeken, & Verschaffel, 2009;Vamvakoussi et al, 2013). Several reaction time studies with student and adult populations support the theories of co-existence and intuitive interference of natural number knowledge by showing statistically significant differences in accuracy rates and response times between tasks that were in-line with intuitions about the results of arithmetic operations and tasks that falsified these intuitions (Obersteiner, Van Hoof, Verschaffel, & Van Dooren, 2016;Vamvakoussi et al, 2012;Van Hoof et al, 2015).…”
supporting
confidence: 68%
“…These questions are particularly relevant in the domain of fraction learning. A large body of work demonstrates that children's prior knowledge of whole numbers negatively biases their understanding of fraction symbols (e.g., Hartnett & Gelman, 1998;Mack, 1995;Meert, Grégoire, & Noël, 2010;Smith, Solomon, & Carey, 2005;Stafylidou & Vosniadou, 2004; and operations (Obersteiner, Van Hoof, Verschaffel, & Van Dooren, 2016;Siegler & Pyke, 2013;Van Hoof, Vandewalle, Verschaffel, & Van Dooren, 2015). This well-documented phenomenon is known as the whole number bias (e.g., Ni & Zhou, 2005).…”
Section: Fractions and The Whole Number Biasmentioning
confidence: 99%