2018
DOI: 10.1017/s1930297500006586
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Who says “larger” and who says “smaller”? Individual differences in the language of comparison

Abstract: When comparing a pair of attribute values, English speakers can use a “larger” comparative (“A is larger/longer/higher/more than B”) or a “smaller” comparative (“B is smaller/shorter/lower/less than A”). This choice matters because it affects people’s inferences about the absolute magnitudes of the compared items, and influences the perceived truthfulness of the comparative sentence itself. In 4 studies (total N = 2335), we investigated the language that people use to describe ordinal relations between attribu… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1

Citation Types

0
2
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

0
3

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(2 citation statements)
references
References 35 publications
(45 reference statements)
0
2
0
Order By: Relevance
“…They lead to more agreement and will more likely be considered true (Hoorens & Bruckmüller, 2015;Zhang & Schwarz, 2020). This asymmetry may reflect an even more general preference for comparing objects in terms of which is larger, higher, stronger, wider, and fuller instead of smaller, lower, weaker, narrower, and emptier (Matthews & Dylman, 2014;Skylark et al, 2018). This "Higher Use of Larger Comparison (HULC)"-effect may in turn be related to the concept of linguistic markedness, which implies a tendency to name dimensions after the upper, "unmarked" end of the dimension (Battistella, 1996;Clark & Clark, 1977).…”
Section: Directionality Againmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…They lead to more agreement and will more likely be considered true (Hoorens & Bruckmüller, 2015;Zhang & Schwarz, 2020). This asymmetry may reflect an even more general preference for comparing objects in terms of which is larger, higher, stronger, wider, and fuller instead of smaller, lower, weaker, narrower, and emptier (Matthews & Dylman, 2014;Skylark et al, 2018). This "Higher Use of Larger Comparison (HULC)"-effect may in turn be related to the concept of linguistic markedness, which implies a tendency to name dimensions after the upper, "unmarked" end of the dimension (Battistella, 1996;Clark & Clark, 1977).…”
Section: Directionality Againmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In language production tasks, people indeed seem to favour "larger" comparatives (Hoorens & Bruckmüller, 2015; see also ) --a so-called higher use of larger comparatives (HULC) effect (Matthews & Dylman, 2014). However, the choice is not arbitrary: whether people use the "smaller" or "larger" comparative to describe a given pair of items depends, inter alia, on the spatial and temporal layout of the objects and on their absolute magnitudes (Matthews & Dylman, 2014;Skylark, 2018); there is also indication that people who are older and those who are more agreeable, conscientious, and emotionally stable are more likely to use "larger" comparatives, although these effects are small (Skylark et al, 2018).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%