Emotion Regulation
DOI: 10.1007/978-0-387-29986-0_6
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Why Crying Improves Our Well-being: An Attachment-Theory Perspective on the Functions of Adult Crying

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

2
44
0
1

Publication Types

Select...
4
4

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 40 publications
(47 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
2
44
0
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Similarly, other theorists emphasize the communicative aspects of crying, considering crying as serving to facilitate attachment and strengthen social bonds by eliciting caregiving and empathetic behaviors in others (e.g., Kottler, 1996;Nelson, 1998;Hendriks, Nelson, Cornelius, & vingerhoets, 2008). Crying has also been shown to be useful in inhibiting aggressive responses (Roes, 1990).…”
Section: Possible Mechanisms For Crying-related Catharsismentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Similarly, other theorists emphasize the communicative aspects of crying, considering crying as serving to facilitate attachment and strengthen social bonds by eliciting caregiving and empathetic behaviors in others (e.g., Kottler, 1996;Nelson, 1998;Hendriks, Nelson, Cornelius, & vingerhoets, 2008). Crying has also been shown to be useful in inhibiting aggressive responses (Roes, 1990).…”
Section: Possible Mechanisms For Crying-related Catharsismentioning
confidence: 96%
“…Consistent with the possibility that mood pathology alters the consequences of crying [Rottenberg et al, 2002], patients in this study reported less mood improvement after crying than their nonpsychiatric counterparts. Theories have proposed that mood improvement after healthy crying is due to endogenous physiological factors associated with crying [increased parasympathetic activity, Rottenberg et al, 2003], or from the success of crying in securing help from the social environment [see Cornelius, 1997, 2001 andHendriks et al, in press for reviews]. Consequently, the finding of reduced mood improvement after depressed crying may reflect physiological deficits, a reduced ability of patients to draw comfort and attention from the social environment, and/or simply that the situations that evoke depressed crying [e.g., memories of loss and failure, Teasdale, 1988] are not as easily corrected as the situations that lead healthy individuals to cry.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Theory suggests that crying communicates that one is vulnerable, suffering, and/or in need of aid (Hendriks, Nelson, et al, 2008) and has benefits for the crier in terms of the empathy, sympathy, pity and/or comfort that it elicits (Buss, 1992;Cornelius, 1997Cornelius, , 2001Hendriks, Croon, et al, 2008;Keltner & Kring, 1998). On this basis, we suspected that crying might be used by DO persons to engage support (i.e., 'instrumental crying; ' Cornelius, 1997' Cornelius, , 2001.…”
Section: Personality and Crying In Relational Contextsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Recall that DO is characterized by a needy, non-reciprocal, and clingy style of relating that likely creates friction inside relationships. Crying in relationships is common (Frey et al, 1983), and conflict and reunion are major contexts for crying (Avery, 1983;Hendriks, Nelson, Cornelius, & Vingerhoets, 2008) that may be differentially common among persons high in DO.…”
Section: Crying: the Link To Personality Functioningmentioning
confidence: 99%