2020
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104449
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Why do human languages have homophones?

Abstract: Human languages are replete with ambiguity. This is most evident in homophony-where two or more words sound the same, but carry distinct meanings. For example, the wordform "bark" can denote either the sound produced by a dog or the protective outer sheath of a tree trunk. Why would a system evolved for efficient, effective communication display rampant ambiguity? Some accounts argue that ambiguity is actually a design feature of human communication systems, allowing languages to recycle their most optimal wor… Show more

Help me understand this report
View preprint versions

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
23
1

Year Published

2020
2020
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 18 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 39 publications
2
23
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Laplace smoothing is a popular choice, being used in a number of recent works in computational linguistics (e.g. Dautriche et al, 2017a;Trott and Bergen, 2020). However, it is perhaps the simplest of such regularisation techniques, and usually leads to much weaker empirical performances than, e.g., Kneyser-Ney (Ney et al, 1994).…”
Section: Experimental Methodologymentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Laplace smoothing is a popular choice, being used in a number of recent works in computational linguistics (e.g. Dautriche et al, 2017a;Trott and Bergen, 2020). However, it is perhaps the simplest of such regularisation techniques, and usually leads to much weaker empirical performances than, e.g., Kneyser-Ney (Ney et al, 1994).…”
Section: Experimental Methodologymentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In this work, we contribute to the debate surrounding a specific form of lexical ambiguity in which a wordform shares multiple unrelated meanings: homophony. 1 While the quantitative study of homophony dates back to (at least) Zipf (1949), recently, Trott and Bergen (2020) proposed a new explanation for the higher rate of homophony amongst good, i.e. short or phonotactically well-formed, wordforms: if words were sampled i.i.d.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Take, for instance, several recent publications in this very journal with titles such as, "Why are social interactions found easily in visual search tasks" (Vestner et al, 2020), "Why do human languages have homophones?" (Trott & Bergen, 2020), and "Gamble evaluation and evoked reference sets: why adding a small loss to a gamble increases its attractiveness" (McKenzie & Sher, 2020). In each of these cases, the authors might intend to ask how it is that some phenomenon arises or what purpose the phenomenon serves in broader cognition.…”
Section: Understanding 'Why'mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…5 Lexical ambiguity allows the mapping of multiple meanings to the same wordform and, in doing so, it enables the preferential re-use of short words (Piantadosi et al, 2012). Thus, the mapping of multiple meanings to the same form could be a source of efficiency in the lexicon (Fenk-Oczlon and Fenk, 2008;Ferrer-i-Cancho and Vitevitch, 2018;Casas et al, 2019;Trott and Bergen, 2020;Xu et al, 2020). Nonetheless, we do not treat it explicitly here.…”
Section: Words As Meaningsmentioning
confidence: 99%