2014
DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.2519125
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Why Do People Keep Their Promises? A Further Investigation

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

3
22
1

Year Published

2017
2017
2021
2021

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 9 publications
(26 citation statements)
references
References 19 publications
3
22
1
Order By: Relevance
“…To shed more light on the possible reasons for which loaded information is more helpful in the prediction task than the neutral one, we now turn to investigating how the content of the target players' statements aects prediction accuracy in the VIDLO treatment. Following a growing body of experimental literature in economics (see, for instance, Charness and Dufwenberg, 2006;Ismayilov and Potters, 2016;Schwartz et al, 2018), our analysis focuses on promise-making as a potential signal of trustworthiness. In the sample of 896 stimuli delivered in VIDLO, 452 contain a promise to Roll (see footnote 3 for details of the coding procedure).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…To shed more light on the possible reasons for which loaded information is more helpful in the prediction task than the neutral one, we now turn to investigating how the content of the target players' statements aects prediction accuracy in the VIDLO treatment. Following a growing body of experimental literature in economics (see, for instance, Charness and Dufwenberg, 2006;Ismayilov and Potters, 2016;Schwartz et al, 2018), our analysis focuses on promise-making as a potential signal of trustworthiness. In the sample of 896 stimuli delivered in VIDLO, 452 contain a promise to Roll (see footnote 3 for details of the coding procedure).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…3 Like Charness and Dufwenberg (2006), we dene a promise as a statement of intent to Roll. Our classication method echoes the recent study by Schwartz et al (2018). All statements were classied as promises or nopromises by two independent coders (research assistants).…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The literature still lacks a common consensus on whether this should involve content analysis carried out by the experimenter (Charness and Dufwenberg, 2006), by independent coders (He et al, 2016), through an incentivized coordination game (Houser and Xiao, 2011), or by asking the subjects for their own interpretation (Servátka et al, 2011). Echoing a recent study by Schwartz et al (2018), herein we implement a method combining the first two approaches. Player Bs' statements were classified as promises or non-promises by two independent coders (research assistants).…”
Section: Summary Of Player Bs' Behaviormentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Females are generally less trusting than males when solely the vebal content of a message is transmitted (which usually does suce to identify the other person's gender). Adding nonverbal 5 See, for instance, Charness and Dufwenberg (2006); Vanberg (2008); Ismayilov and Potters (2016);Schwartz et al (2019). For a systematic review of the experimental literature on promise-making, see Woike and Kanngiesser (2019).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%