2014
DOI: 10.1111/brv.12134
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Why ratio dependence is (still) a bad model of predation

Abstract: The history of the idea that predation rates are functions of the ratio of prey density to predator density, known as ratio dependence, is reviewed and updated. When the term was introduced in 1989, it was already known that higher predator abundance often reduced an individual predator's average intake rate of prey. However, the idea that this effect was a universally applicable inverse proportionality was new. That idea was widely criticized in many articles in the early 1990s, and many of these criticisms h… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
2
1

Citation Types

0
67
0

Year Published

2014
2014
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
7

Relationship

0
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 50 publications
(67 citation statements)
references
References 177 publications
(346 reference statements)
0
67
0
Order By: Relevance
“…We recognize, however, that ratio-dependent functional responses remain an area of contention in ecology, with critics cautioning that use of relatively simple models necessarily ignore the influence of factors such as movement, landscape heterogeneity, and prey behavior, on predation rates. Such real-world complexities may lead to indirect interference (i.e., pseudo-interference) even in the absence of direct interactions between predators (Free et al 1977, Abrams and Walters 1996, Abrams 2015. It is unlikely that these functional responses are wholly prey or ratio dependent, but exhibit some degree of intermediate interference (Arditi and Akc ßakaya 1990).…”
Section: July 2017mentioning
confidence: 99%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…We recognize, however, that ratio-dependent functional responses remain an area of contention in ecology, with critics cautioning that use of relatively simple models necessarily ignore the influence of factors such as movement, landscape heterogeneity, and prey behavior, on predation rates. Such real-world complexities may lead to indirect interference (i.e., pseudo-interference) even in the absence of direct interactions between predators (Free et al 1977, Abrams and Walters 1996, Abrams 2015. It is unlikely that these functional responses are wholly prey or ratio dependent, but exhibit some degree of intermediate interference (Arditi and Akc ßakaya 1990).…”
Section: July 2017mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Interestingly, the extent to which predator‐ or ratio‐dependent functional responses outperform prey‐dependent models appears to vary across systems (Arditi and Ginzburg , Abrams ). Of the few empirical tests assessing the variety of functional responses, it seems that, depending on the system, prey‐dependent (Arditi and Saïah ), ratio‐dependent (Vucetich et al.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…As stressed by van der Meer & Ens (), most of these models are phenomenological because they lack a mechanistic underpinning of the processes of prey and competitor encounter, rendering it difficult to use them as firm building blocks in follow‐up studies. Unfortunately, ratio‐dependent predation models, which have been claimed to offer an altered perspective on trophic ecology (Arditi & Ginzburg ), are of phenomenological nature too (Abrams ). But, note that even the few generalized functional response models that do mechanistically include competition have significant drawbacks.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…An alternative explanation for the weak support for predator dependence in the caging experiment is that the cages, or their placement within an early successional age patch that was dominated by a single barnacle species, altered whelk foraging behaviour from that exhibited across the sets of surveyed patches more generally. This interpretation may seem to challenge the concern that traditional functional response experiments involving isolated predator–prey pairs could be favoring the detection of predator dependence by selecting for strong predator–prey interactions (Abrams ). However, the results of our analysis are also consistent with this concern in that N. ostrina 's estimated interference rate (Fig.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…; Perretti et al . ; Abrams ). Indeed, all methods for quantifying the strengths and hence importance of species interactions make assumptions regarding their functional form (Wootton & Emmerson ; Vázquez et al .…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%