1985
DOI: 10.3758/bf03213369
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Within- and between-subjects partial reinforcement effects with an autoshaped response using Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica)

Abstract: Animals exposed to schedules of partial reinforcement are typically more resistant to extinction than are animals trained with continuous reinforcement. This is the partial reinforcement effect (PRE). Animals experienced with both partial and continuous schedules are often more persistent on the continuous schedule, yielding a reversed PRE. Both conventional and reversed PREs have been elusive with classical conditioning paradigms. The present experiment attempted to demonstrate between-and within-subject PREs… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1

Citation Types

7
18
0

Year Published

1990
1990
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
8
1

Relationship

0
9

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 32 publications
(25 citation statements)
references
References 18 publications
7
18
0
Order By: Relevance
“…If one replots their data by reinforcer, there is some evidence of superiority in the partially reinforced conditions; however, such replotting necessarily consists of a single data point with no statistical evaluation. Crawford, Steirn, and Pavlik (1985), using Japanese quail, compared 100% and 50% reinforcement, plotting their results by trial, and not by reinforcer. If one replots their data, there is no obvious difference in initial acquisition and an asymptotic difference in favor of a partially reinforced group in both rate and percent CR measures.…”
Section: Empirical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…If one replots their data by reinforcer, there is some evidence of superiority in the partially reinforced conditions; however, such replotting necessarily consists of a single data point with no statistical evaluation. Crawford, Steirn, and Pavlik (1985), using Japanese quail, compared 100% and 50% reinforcement, plotting their results by trial, and not by reinforcer. If one replots their data, there is no obvious difference in initial acquisition and an asymptotic difference in favor of a partially reinforced group in both rate and percent CR measures.…”
Section: Empirical Backgroundmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…However, a more traditional measure of strength of keypeck responding-rate of maintained responding-shows less sensitivity to these variables (Balsam & Payne, 1979;Crawford, Steirn, & Pavlik, 1985;Gonzalez, 1974;Kay, Hemmes, & Brown, 1984;Lucas, Deich, & Wasserman, 1981; Newlin & LoLordo, 1976;O'Connell & Rashotte, 1982;Perkins et al, 1975).…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Since the work of Amsel and Roussel (1952), psychologists have found that Pavlovian responses are magnified when cues are unreliable predictors of reward. In Pavlovian autoshaping, signtracking often comes to reach a higher asymptotic level when a CS is unsystematically followed by a UCS than when a CS is always followed by a UCS (Amsel et al 1964;Boakes 1977;Collins et al 1983;Crawford et al 1985;Gibbon et al 1980;Gottlieb 2004;Robinson et al 2014;Swan & Pearce 1987;Torres et al 2016). The effect is easy to replicate, although some studies failed to obtain it (e.g., Papini & Overmier 1984;1985;Rescorla 1999).…”
Section: Sign-tracking and Uncertaintymentioning
confidence: 99%