2015
DOI: 10.1111/psyp.12514
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

You see what you have learned. Evidence for an interrelation of associative learning and visual selective attention

Abstract: Besides visual salience and observers' current intention, prior learning experience may influence deployment of visual attention. Associative learning models postulate that observers pay more attention to stimuli previously experienced as reliable predictors of specific outcomes. To investigate the impact of learning experience on deployment of attention, we combined an associative learning task with a visual search task and measured event-related potentials of the EEG as neural markers of attention deployment… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1

Citation Types

20
90
0

Year Published

2016
2016
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
10

Relationship

4
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 87 publications
(110 citation statements)
references
References 51 publications
20
90
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Consistent with this idea, Le Pelley et al (2013) demonstrated that providing more time for participants to consciously process the stimuli-by increasing the SOA on dot probe trials to 1000 ms-significantly weakened the influence of predictiveness on dot probe responding. This supports the idea that the bias towards predictive cues observed in the short SOA condition was not a result of goal-directed, controlled processing but instead an automatic, rapid, and short-lived attentional process within the region of 250 milliseconds after cue-onset (for convergent evidence, see Feldmann-Wüstefeld, Uengoer, & Schubö, 2015). …”
supporting
confidence: 69%
“…Consistent with this idea, Le Pelley et al (2013) demonstrated that providing more time for participants to consciously process the stimuli-by increasing the SOA on dot probe trials to 1000 ms-significantly weakened the influence of predictiveness on dot probe responding. This supports the idea that the bias towards predictive cues observed in the short SOA condition was not a result of goal-directed, controlled processing but instead an automatic, rapid, and short-lived attentional process within the region of 250 milliseconds after cue-onset (for convergent evidence, see Feldmann-Wüstefeld, Uengoer, & Schubö, 2015). …”
supporting
confidence: 69%
“…However, no difference was found between participants that were aware and unaware of the reward assignment. Thirdly, the N D and P D observed for distractors resembled in shape and timing of N D and P D components that were found in similar paradigms in which no reward or other incentive was used to render salient distractors objects of potential interest (Feldmann‐Wüstefeld & Schubö, ; Feldmann‐Wüstefeld, Uengoer, & Schubö, ). In sum, it seems that voluntary attention deployment in a top‐down manner toward rewarded distractors may not explain the present pattern of results, and neither do differences in bottom‐up processing (as colors were counterbalanced across participants).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 79%
“…This fast suppression is consistent with recent studies that claimed that suppression of salient singletons in the additional singleton task can be feature-based (Gaspelin, Leonard, & Luck, 2015; Moher & Egeth, 2012; Sawaki & Luck, 2010; Vatterott & Vecera, 2012). Such fast suppression can also take place at the level of whole perceptual dimensions (Feldmann-Wüstefeld, Uengoer, & Schubö, 2015). Alternatively, the results from Experiment 2 could reflect the unresolved conflict between reward- and goal-driven attentional biases, possibly resulting in an impaired performance at the ISI 100-ms condition for both reward- and goal-congruent trials.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%