2018
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.12.004
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Young infants expect an unfamiliar adult to comfort a crying baby: Evidence from a standard violation-of-expectation task and a novel infant-triggered-video task

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1

Citation Types

3
31
1

Year Published

2019
2019
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
6
2

Relationship

3
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 69 publications
(35 citation statements)
references
References 77 publications
3
31
1
Order By: Relevance
“…The presence of selective expectations about indirect TPP in toddlers and infants gives weight to the notion that these expectations reflect an abstract principle of ingroup support, which is part of the "first draft" (8) of human moral cognition (4-10, 57, 59, 60). As noted in the Introduction, from a very young age, children's concern for ingroup support carries a rich set of expectations related to caring for ingroup members and showing them loyalty (57)(58)(59)(60)(61)(62)(63)69). Our research extends these findings by demonstrating that just as children expect individuals to refrain from harming ingroup members, they also expect individuals to punish, at least indirectly, harm to ingroup members, whether perpetrated by ingroup or outgroup wrongdoers.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…The presence of selective expectations about indirect TPP in toddlers and infants gives weight to the notion that these expectations reflect an abstract principle of ingroup support, which is part of the "first draft" (8) of human moral cognition (4-10, 57, 59, 60). As noted in the Introduction, from a very young age, children's concern for ingroup support carries a rich set of expectations related to caring for ingroup members and showing them loyalty (57)(58)(59)(60)(61)(62)(63)69). Our research extends these findings by demonstrating that just as children expect individuals to refrain from harming ingroup members, they also expect individuals to punish, at least indirectly, harm to ingroup members, whether perpetrated by ingroup or outgroup wrongdoers.…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 67%
“…Finally, our results make clear that these changes in children's expectations occur only with transgressions against ingroup victims. When the victim was not specifically identified as a member of the bystander's group, toddlers' and infants' expectations were those typically found for interactions between ingroup individuals (57)(58)(59)(60)(61)(62)(63) or between noningroup individuals (46, 53-55, 57-63, 69, 70). Thus, in the case of an ingroup wrongdoer, the bystander was expected to provide help and to refrain from hindering; in the case of a noningroup wrongdoer, however, helping and hindering were both viewed as acceptable courses of action.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…The distribution of these 15 infants, in terms of the condition they were assigned to (and the test event they saw), was: Experiment 1, 4 experimental (1 unequal, 3 equal) and 3 inanimate-control (1 unequal, 2 equal); Experiment 2, 3 experimental (equal) and 3 cover-control (1 unequal, 2 equal); and Experiment 3, 1 experimental (equal) and 1 control (unequal). Ceiling infants are typically eliminated on the assumption that they needed additional familiarization to process the events they were shown (for other reports with eliminated ceiling babies, see e.g., Scott et al, 2015; Baillargeon and DeJong, 2017; Jin et al, 2018; Margoni et al, 2018). In line with this assumption, the ceiling infants in Experiments 1–3 looked significantly longer during the familiarization trial ( n = 15, M = 33.77, SD = 20.62) than did the infants included in the experiments ( n = 120, M = 18.24, SD = 12.59), F (1,133) = 17.24, p < 0.0001.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%
“…This suggested that infants would perceive the wrongdoer and the victim, 2 group members of equal standing, to be entitled to an equal share of the toys, and hence that they would detect a fairness transgression when the wrongdoer took both toys, leaving none for the victim. Finally, there is also evidence that infants aged 12 to 17 mo possess an abstract expectation of ingroup support: For example, individuals in a group are expected to care for each other by refraining from unprovoked harm and by providing assistance when needed (47)(48)(49)(50). This suggested that infants would also perceive an ingroup-support transgression in our events and would be particularly sensitive to the plight of the victim, who was treated unfairly by a member of her own group.…”
Section: The Present Researchmentioning
confidence: 98%