2017
DOI: 10.1111/cdev.12888
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

Young Infants’ Word Comprehension Given An Unfamiliar Talker or Altered Pronunciations

Abstract: To understand spoken words, listeners must appropriately interpret co-occurring talker characteristics and speech sound content. This ability was tested in 6- to 14-months-olds by measuring their looking to named food and body part images. In the new talker condition (n = 90), pictures were named by an unfamiliar voice; in the mispronunciation condition (n = 98), infants' mothers "mispronounced" the words (e.g., nazz for nose). Six- to 7-month-olds fixated target images above chance across conditions, understa… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
3
1

Citation Types

0
52
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2024
2024

Publication Types

Select...
7
3

Relationship

3
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 37 publications
(52 citation statements)
references
References 42 publications
0
52
0
Order By: Relevance
“…The present results suggest that while infants hear most of their input from their mothers, they also hear several other speakers during high talk‐volume times, which in turn feed infants’ word‐form representations. Indeed, recent lab studies have found that at this age, infants look equivalently to named images when words are reproduced by a new person or their mother (Bergelson & Swingley, ), suggesting some degree of cross‐talker normalization by ~6 months. In contrast, 14‐month‐olds’ learning of similar‐sounding words improves after training with tokens from multiple speakers (Rost & McMurray, ), suggesting that even small amounts of talker variability aid new learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…The present results suggest that while infants hear most of their input from their mothers, they also hear several other speakers during high talk‐volume times, which in turn feed infants’ word‐form representations. Indeed, recent lab studies have found that at this age, infants look equivalently to named images when words are reproduced by a new person or their mother (Bergelson & Swingley, ), suggesting some degree of cross‐talker normalization by ~6 months. In contrast, 14‐month‐olds’ learning of similar‐sounding words improves after training with tokens from multiple speakers (Rost & McMurray, ), suggesting that even small amounts of talker variability aid new learning.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 94%
“…For instance, in light of these results, theories of phonological development may consider more heavily that infants’ native language phonemic representations do not sample from the full range of speaker variability, but are instead dominated by adult female speakers’ phonemes, which themselves are more CDS‐heavy than the (minority) input from male adults (cf. Bergelson & Swingley, ; Martin et al., ). Indeed work with slightly older children has found differences in phonetic realizations of sociolinguistic variants in CDS that differ from those in ADS (Foulkes, Docherty, & Watt, ).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…The ability to generalize across speakers, genders, or emotions emerges around 10.5 months of age [47,48], with evidence of infants' ability to generalize across accents emerging around their first birthday [49,50]. Related work on infant word recognition suggests that infants between 8 and 10 months old might be particularly negatively impacted by speech variation [51]. Thus, it is conceivable that the infants up to one year of age are not yet able to transfer words learned from song to speech.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%