I Abstract Durkheim hypothesized that basic categories of thought are based on society as their model, and that these mental representations arise from participation in society's totemic rites. This proposition is evaluated in light of recent research on the cognitive architecture of newborns and infants. The evidence suggests that presocialized infants possess mental representations of not only the physical world but also the minds of others and even the sui generis quality of collectivities. This review concludes that the Durkheimian theory of the social origin of mind has little empirical support and suggests that the sociology of mental life needs to be radically retheorized.
Theories of international terrorism are reviewed. It then is noted that waves of terrorism appear in semiperipheral zones of the world-system during pulsations of globalization when the dominant state is in decline. Finally, how these and other factors might combine to suggest a model of terrorism's role in the cyclical undulations of the world-system is suggested.
s model of language and mind, while perhaps understandable at the time it was written, now seems inadequate. First, the research evidence strongly suggests that mental operations exist prior to language onset, conversation of gestures, or social interaction. Second, language is not just significant symbols; it requires syntax. Third, syntax seems to be part of our bioinheritance, that is, part of our presocial mind/brain-what Noam Chomsky has called our language faculty. Fourth, this means syntax probably is not learned nor a social construction that is internalized as a cultural template. Fifth, this suggests a basic reversal of the prevailing model of symbolic interaction, mind, language, and perhaps the self as well, although there has not been the time or space to engage that topic here. Therefore, symbolic interaction may turn out to be a more Chomskyan than Meadian process. Given the bioinheritance of our mind/brain we are able to engage in symbolic interaction; it does not appear that symbolic interaction creates our mind or the basic computational algorithms of language.*I would like to thank Jonathan Turner and the anonymous reviewers for Sociological Theory for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article. Direct correspondence to:
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.Bernhard Giesen examines German national identity through history and divides it into several periods. The strength of the book is his description of themes in German national at different points in time. He covers what he calls the "patriotic code," the "code of Romanticism," the "code of Realpolitik," and concludes with a discussion of the German identity between 1945 and 1990. The differences are interesting and informative and constitute the substantive meat of the book.The explanatory sociology, though, is a little thinner. Giesen's central claim is that through "the communication rituals specific to intellectuals . . . there arise-mostly without conscious intention, and in a manner shaped by the specific logic of the intellectual community the ideas of the 'general,' the Allgemeinheit, the encompassing collectivity that lies in the other worldly order, i.e., the very ideas that define codes of collective identity" (p. 45). This puts a big burden upon a rather small class of individuals (intellectuals) as the source of a very large entity (German national identity).But the explanation is even narrower, for Giesen claims it is the discourse patterns involved in how intellectuals associate that holds the key to understanding the underlying codes of national identity. "Inasmuch as the free association of individuals in the Verein [clubs and associations] serves as a model for modern socialization, this form of communication also fosters the idea of history as progress through free and reasonable action" (p. 72). But an idea such as "history as progress" seems to have origins somewhere else besides the free associational character of German clubs and associations. Its not only a very old, but a very big idea, and its adoption, acceptance, or institutionalization would seem to have more to do with historical class formations, political structures, or deeprooted cultural traditions than with the rise of clubs and associations. Such an interactional explanation is just too narrow and light to carry the explanatory width and macro weight of such IrLtellectuals and the Nation: Collective Identity in a German Axial Age, by Bernhard Giesen. Translated by Nicholas Levis and Amso Weisz. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998. 246 pp. $64.95 cloth. ISBN: 0-521-62161-5. $24.95 paper. ISBN: 0-521-63996-4.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2025 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.