In this study, we employed an eye-gaze paradigm to explore whether children (8-12) and adolescents (12-18) with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are able to use prosodic cues to determine the syntactic structure of an utterance. Persons with ASD were compared to typicallydeveloping (TD) peers matched on age, IQ, gender, and receptive language abilities. The stimuli were syntactically ambiguous but had a prosodic break that indicated the appropriate interpretation (feel the frog…with the feather vs. feel…the frog with the feather). We found that all groups were equally sensitive to the initial prosodic cues that were presented. Children and teens with ASD used prosody to interpret the ambiguous phrase as rapidly and efficiently as their TD peers. However, when a different cue was presented in subsequent trials, the younger ASD group was more likely to respond in a manner consistent with the initial prosodic cue rather than the new one. Eye-tracking data indicated that both younger groups (ASD and TD) had trouble shifting their interpretation as the prosodic cue changed, but the younger TD group was able to overcome this interference and produce an action consistent with the prosodic cue.KEYWORDS: Autism, prosody, intonation, syntax, communication The Use of Prosody During Syntactic Processing in Children With Autism Spectrum DisordersAutism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) are neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by deficits in social interaction and communication, along with a propensity to engage in repetitive behaviors or have restricted interests (APA, 2000). The severity of these deficits and the ways in which they are expressed vary considerably. Until recently, most children diagnosed with ASD had severe language impairments or delays, and researchers estimated that as many as half were non-verbal (Lord & Paul, 1997). However, more recent estimates suggest that 80-86% of children with ASDs have some functional language (Lord, Risi, & Pickles, 2004). In fact, a substantial proportion of the school-aged children with ASD do not appear to have deficits in vocabulary, articulation or syntax (Joseph, Tager-Flusberg & Lord, 2002; Kjelgaard & TagerFlusberg, 2001. We will be referring to children with this profile as highly verbal.There are, however, two domains of language that appear to be impaired even in highlyverbal children with ASD. First, persons with ASD have impairments in pragmatics (Kelley, Paul, Fein & Naigles, 2006;Tager-Flusberg, Paul, & Lord, 2005;Young, Diehl, Morris, Hyman, & Bennetto, 2005), that seem related to their deficits in social interaction. Pragmatics represents the skills that allow us to use language as a social tool by going beyond the literal meaning of an utterance to understand the role that it plays in a particular interaction. Highly-verbal persons with ASD often perform well on highly structured measures of pragmatic ability (Young et al., 2005). Nonetheless, children with this profile have been found to have deficits in: interpreting the conversational intentions (and som...
During communication, conversational partners should offer as much information as is required and relevant. For instance, the statement "Some Xs Y" is infelicitous if one knows that all Xs Y. Do children understand the link between speaker knowledge and utterance strength? In Experiment 1, 5-year-olds (N = 32) but not 4-year-olds (N = 32) reliably connected statements of different logical strength (e.g., "The girl colored all/some of the star") to observers who were fully or partially informed. Four-year-olds' performance improved when observer knowledge could be assessed more easily (Experiment 2a, N = 25) but remained the same in a nonlinguistic version of Experiment 1 that preserved the epistemic requirements of the original study (Experiment 2b, N = 26). These findings have implications for the development of early communicative abilities.
During communication, hearers try to infer the speaker's intentions to be able to understand what the speaker means. Nevertheless, whether (and how early) preschoolers track their interlocutors' mental states is still a matter of debate. Furthermore, there is disagreement about how children's ability to consult a speaker's belief in communicative contexts relates to their ability to track someone's belief in non-communicative contexts. Here, we study young children's ability to successfully acquire a word from a speaker with a false belief; we also assess the same children's success on a traditional false belief attribution task. We show that the ability to consult the epistemic state of a speaker during word learning develops between the ages of three and five. We also show that false belief understanding in word-learning contexts proceeds similarly to standard belief-attribution contexts when the tasks are equated. Our data offer evidence for the development of mind-reading abilities during language acquisition.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.