Remembering is a reconstructive process, yet little is known about how the reconstruction of a memory unfolds in time in the human brain. Here, we used reaction times and EEG time-series decoding to test the hypothesis that the information flow is reversed when an event is reconstructed from memory, compared to when the same event is initially being perceived. Across three experiments, we found highly consistent evidence supporting such a reversed stream. When seeing an object, low-level perceptual features were discriminated faster behaviourally, and could be decoded from brain activity earlier, than high-level conceptual features. This pattern reversed during associative memory recall, with reaction times and brain activity patterns now indicating that conceptual information was reconstructed more rapidly than perceptual details. Our findings support a neurobiologically plausible model of human memory, suggesting that memory retrieval is a hierarchical, multi-layered process that prioritises semantically meaningful information over perceptual details.
Highlights d We show evidence for theta phase encoding-retrieval models in human EEG data d Neural indices of memory reactivation from pattern classifiers fluctuate at 7 or 8 Hz d Times of maximal memory reactivation are preceded by a consistent theta phase d Optimal phase for encoding and retrieval is shifted by approximately 180
When investigating local sources in human S-EEG, ICA should be preferred over re-referencing the data with a bipolar montage.
Human thought is highly flexible, achieved by evolving patterns of brain activity across groups of cells. Neuroscience aims to understand cognition in the brain by analysing these intricate patterns. We argue this goal is impeded by the time format of our dataclock time. The brain is a system with its own dynamics and regime of time, with no intrinsic concern for the human-invented second. Here, we present the Brain Time Toolbox, a software library that retunes electrophysiology data in line with oscillations that orchestrate neural patterns of cognition. These oscillations continually slow down, speed up, and undergo abrupt changes, introducing a disharmony between the brain's internal regime and clock time. The toolbox overcomes this disharmony by warping the data to the dynamics of coordinating oscillations, setting oscillatory cycles as the data's new time axis. This enables the study of neural patterns as they unfold in the brain, aiding neuroscientific inquiry into dynamic cognition. In support of this, we demonstrate that the toolbox can reveal results that are absent in a default clock time format. Studying dynamic cognitionEveryday tasks involve a plethora of cognitive functions that operate dynamically in tandem. Something as mundane as taking notes during a meeting or battling your friend in a video game requires attention, motor activity, perception, memory, and decision-making, each evolving over time. How does the brain achieve dynamic cognition? To answer this question, neuroscientists closely study how brain activity unfolds from one moment to the next using temporally precise neuroimaging methods. These include electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), and single and multi-unit recordingsgrouped together under the term electrophysiology. Seconds are foreign to the brainIn a typical electrophysiology study, neuroscientists first probe cognition by introducing an experimental manipulation. For example, an attention researcher might introduce a set of moving dots. Then, to understand cognition in the brain, they perform a series of analyses on the recorded data. They might study changes in scalp topography over a second of data, apply machine learning to characterize how the representation of the dots evolves, or perform any other time-dependent analysis.Critically, from the raw output of neuroimaging devices to the analysis of recorded brain signals, time is operationalized as clock timesequences of milliseconds. We claim that clock time, with all its benefits for human affairs, is generally inappropriate for neuroscience. This is because clock time is defined by us and for us, based on how long it takes for Earth to rotate its axis. The brain itself, however, employs its own regime of time, dictated by its own dynamics. As such, the brain is indifferent to how many milliseconds, seconds, minutes, or hours have passed unless it is expressly relevant for specific behaviour, such as maintaining circadian rhythms [1] or tracking a time-dependent reward [2]. Instead, the brain is conc...
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.