This report is based upon a dissertation submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Michigan in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Ph.D. degree.
Ratings imply comparative judgments between the values of the observer and his observations, for instance, a student's ratings of his teacher are estimates of the discrepancy between the student's ideals for the teacher's behavior and what he sees the teacher do. However, most methods for collecting teacher ratings make assumptions about ideals and about the discrepancies between ideals and observed behavior. In order to assess the relevance of direct measurement of ideals, students were asked to rate a teacher and to report their ideals for the teacher's behaviors. The judgments of ideal behavior varied across students and items; there were also interactions between ideals and observed responses. Thus, the authors recommend new approaches to teacher ratings.Students have ideas about ideal teacher behaviors. Students also see their teachers behave. One way to interpret students' evaluations of teachers is to say that each student estimates the discrepancy between his ideal for that behavior and what he sees his instructor do. When the instructor's behavior comes close to the student's ideal, the teacher receives a high rating; if the teacher is far from the student's ideal, the teacher receives a low rating.Most approaches to teacher evaluation do not assess the student's ideal directly. For example, students may be asked to rate the teacher's "flexibility" on a 5-point scale ranging from "outstanding" to "poor." In this approach, the student makes an explicit evaluation (i.e., estimates a discrepancy) based upon an implicit observation. The response of "poor" for a given attribute would be interpreted as the maximum discrepancy between the ideal and observed behavior for each student. However, the extent to which similar low ratings among a set of students truly reflects the constancy of the actual 'This paper is an expanded version of one presented at the annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association in Chicago, 1969. 1 Requests for reprints should be sent to Leonard M. Lansky, Department of Psychology, McMicken Hall, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, Ohio 45221.psychological discrepancies depends upon the assumption that all students have the same ideal point along the attribute dimension.Coombs (1964) has discussed the problem of interpreting such evaluations. He has suggested that students stretch their maximum discrepancy distances to fit their evaluation scales. Figure 1 contains a hypothetical distribution of ideal and observed responses on a rating scale for "flexibility." If, for example, Student A has an ideal point at I A at the middle of the distribution of possible observations, the maximum discrepancy can be only about half as great as that of Student B, having his ideal point at IB, far to the left end of the observation distribution. A rating of "poor" by Student B would represent a much greater discrepancy between his ideal and his observation than a rating of "poor" by Student A, even though both students made the same rating response. According to this interpretation, individ...
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.