Learned behavior varies in its resistance to change, depending on the rate of reinforcement. Resistance to change may be characterized as behavioral momentum, which in turn may be analyzed into terms corresponding to mass and velocity in classical physics. Behavioral mass may be inferred from changes in response rate when experimental conditions are altered. Relevant data were obtained by training pigeons to peck a key on two-component multiple variable-interval, variable-interval schedules. Six pigeons were studied on three pairs of variable-interval schedules in all possible orders. When performance stabilized, resistance to change was assessed by arranging response-independent food during periods between components and by extinction. For each operation, the data for all schedule performances converged onto a single function, permitting estimation of the ratio of behavioral masses for each pair of schedules. The response-independent food data suggested that the ratio of behavioral masses is a power function of the ratio of reinforcement rates and that behavioral mass may be measured on a ratio scale.
Key pecking by pigeons was maintained on chained schedules of food reinforcement. Within each session, two chained schedules with identical initial links but different terminal-link conditions of reinforcement alternated irregularly on two keys. Across successive experimental conditions, the duration of access to food (Experiment 1) or the length of the terminal link and the duration of access to food (Experiment 2) were varied systematically. Asymptotic response rates were determined in each condition, and resistance to change was assessed by arranging alternative reinforcement on a third key or by prefeeding. Response rates were lower and responding was always less resistant to change in the initial links than in the terminal links. Response rate and resistance to change in both links of a chain increased when the duration of access to food increased or the length of the terminal link decreased. The results of both experiments suggested that relative asymptotic response rate and relative resistance to change in both links of the chains depended on a single variable, relative total rate of access to food per unit of terminal-link time.This research was supported in part by Grants BNS 74-11849 and BNS 76-11028 from the National Science Foundation to the Univeristy of New Hampshire. Jean Atak and Peter Jenkins helped to conduct the experiments and contributed valuable insights. Portions of the data reported here were presented by the first two authors at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, April 1977. We are indebted to Carol Eckerman (Reference Note 1) for making her data available to us.
Reinforcement was introduced for responses normally treated as errors in signal-detection procedures. The first experiment used a standard two-response discrete-trial procedure with no reinforcement for errors. Results showed that rats altered their response biases but maintained constant sensitivity to visual signals when reinforcement probabilities varied, and that their sensitivity depended on the physical difference between signals, in accordance with the predictions of signal-detection theory. Experiment II, with rats, and Experiment III, with pigeons, demonstrated that sensitivity decreased in this procedure when reinforcement was scheduled for errors with the signals held constant, despite independence of overall number of reinforcers and sensitivity. Experiment IV, with rats, replicated the decrease in sensitivity in a continuous procedure employing only one response. The decrements in sensitivity were similar across Experiments II, III, and IV, and accorded well with earlier research. Thus, contrary to a fundamental assumption of signal-detection theory, estimates of sensitivity are not always invariant with respect to the outcomes of responding, but depend on relative reinforcement of correct responses.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.