Does serial learning result in specific associations between pairs of items, or does it result in a cognitive map based on relations of all items? In 2 experiments, we trained human participants to learn various lists of photographic images. We then tested the participants on new lists of photographic images. These new lists were constructed by selecting only 1 image from each list learned during training. In Experiment 1, participants were trained to choose the earlier (experimenter defined) item when presented with adjacent pairs of items on each of 5 different 5-item lists. Participants were then tested on derived lists, in which each item retained its original ordinal position, even though each of the presented pairs was novel. Participants performed above chance on all of the derived lists. In Experiment 2, a different group of participants received the same training as those of Experiment 1, but the ordinal positions of items were systematically changed on each derived list. The response accuracy for Experiment 2 varied inversely with the degree to which an item's original ordinal position was changed. These results can be explained by a model in which participants learned to make both positional inferences about the absolute rank of each stimulus, and transitive inferences about the relative ranks of pairs of stimuli. These inferences enhanced response accuracy when ordinal position was maintained, but not when it was changed. Our results demonstrate quantitatively that, in addition to item-item associations that participants acquire while learning a list of arbitrary items, they form a cognitive map that represents both experienced and inferred relationships.
For more than 100 years, psychologists have struggled to determine what is learned during serial learning. The method of derived lists is a powerful tool for studying this question. In two experiments, we trained human participants to learn implicit lists by the Transitive Inference (TI) method. We then tested their knowledge of ordinal position of those items. In Experiment 1, participants were presented with pairs of photographic stimuli from five different 5-item training lists by presenting adjacent pairs of items from one list on every trial. Participants were then tested on pairs of items drawn from different lists, in which each item maintained its original ordinal position as it had during training. In Experiment 2, a different group of participants was trained on the same five 5-item lists as that of Experiment 1. However, the order of the items in the derived lists of Experiment 2 was changed systematically. In this latter experiment, the acquisition rate for the derived lists varied inversely with the degree to which ordinal position was changed. We explain these results by using a model in which participants learn to make positional, as well as transitive inferences, allowing them to infer the relative and absolute position of each item during testing on derived lists.
The implied order of a ranked set of visual images can be learned by transitive inference, without reliance on stimulus features that explicitly signal their order. Such learning is difficult to explain by associative mechanisms but can be accounted for by cognitive representations and processes such as transitive inference. Our study seeks to determine if those representations may be applied to categories of images without explicit verbal instruction. Specifically, we asked whether participants can (a) infer that images being presented belonged to familiar categories, even when every image presented during every trial is unique, and (b) perform transitive inferences about the ordering of those categories. To address these questions, we compared the performance of humans during a standard TI task, which used the same set of images throughout the session, to performance in a category TI tasks, which drew images from a set of categories. Each of the images used in the category TI task was only presented once, limiting the extent to which stimulus-outcome associations could be learned. Participants were able to learn the order of the categories based on transitive inference. However, participants in the category TI condition did not produce a symbolic distance effect. These findings collectively suggest that differing cognitive processes may underpin serial learning when learning about specific stimuli versus stimulus categories.
The implied order of a ranked set of visual images can be learned by transitive inference, without reliance on stimulus features that explicitly signal their order. Such learning is difficult to explain by associative mechanisms but can be accounted for by cognitive representations and processes such as transitive inference. Our study seeks to determine if those representations may be applied to categories of images without explicit verbal instruction. Specifically, we asked whether participants can (a) infer that images being presented belonged to familiar categories, even when every image presented during every trial is unique, and (b) perform transitive inferences about the ordering of those categories. To address these questions, we compared the performance of humans during a standard TI task, which used the same set of images throughout the session, to performance in a category TI tasks, which drew images from a set of categories. Each of the images used in the category TI task was only presented once, limiting the extent to which stimulus-outcome associations could be learned. Participants were able to learn the order of the categories based on transitive inference. However, participants in the category TI condition did not produce a symbolic distance effect. These findings collectively suggest that differing cognitive processes may underpin serial learning when learning about specific stimuli versus stimulus categories.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2025 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.