Echolocation is an active sense enabling bats and toothed whales to orient in darkness through echo returns from their ultrasonic signals. Immediately before prey capture, both bats and whales emit a buzz with such high emission rates (≥180 Hz) and overall duration so short that its functional significance remains an enigma. To investigate sensory-motor control during the buzz of the insectivorous bat Myotis daubentonii, we removed prey, suspended in air or on water, before expected capture. The bats responded by shortening their echolocation buzz gradually; the earlier prey was removed down to approximately 100 ms (30 cm) before expected capture, after which the full buzz sequence was emitted both in air and over water. Bats trawling over water also performed the full capture behavior, but in-air capture motions were aborted, even at very late prey removals (<20 ms = 6 cm before expected contact). Thus, neither the buzz nor capture movements are stereotypical, but dynamically adapted based on sensory feedback. The results indicate that echolocation is controlled mainly by acoustic feedback, whereas capture movements are adjusted according to both acoustic and somatosensory feedback, suggesting separate (but coordinated) central motor control of the two behaviors based on multimodal input. Bat echolocation, especially the terminal buzz, provides a unique window to extremely fast decision processes in response to sensory feedback and modulation through attention in a naturally behaving animal.ost sensory systems passively sample the environment by relying on extrinsic energy sources like light or sound to stimulate sensory receptors. Truly active senses, e.g., the electric sense of weakly electric fishes (1) and echolocation (2), where the animal itself produces the energy used to probe the surroundings, are rare (3). The advanced echolocation systems of bats and toothed whales involve dynamic adaptation of the outgoing sound and behavior based on perception of the surroundings through information processing of returning echoes.The temporal pattern of echolocation signals during prey pursuit changes through three phases: search, approach, and terminal buzz. The buzz, immediately preceding prey capture, is characterized by a dramatic increase in signal repetition rate and is universally present in both bats and whales capturing moving prey (4-8). Repetition rates up to 640 Hz have been reported for porpoises and, contrary to bats, odontocete buzzes usually continue beyond prey contact (6). The buzz of many vespertilionid and molossid bats has two distinct subphases: buzz I with decreasing call durations and intervals, followed by buzz II, with a constant maximum call repetition rate and a characteristic frequency drop of up to an octave (4,(9)(10)(11)(12)(13)(14).The function of the terminal buzz is still not understood (15). It has been hypothesized that odontocete buzzes not only track prey before capture (7), but may also serve to follow escaping prey (6). Bat buzzes have also been hypothesized to help track ev...
The auditory system measures time with exceptional precision. Echolocating bats evaluate the time delay between call and echo to measure object range. An extreme and disputed result on ranging acuity was found in the virtual delay jitter experiments. In these studies, echoes with alternating delays were played back to bats, which detected a jitter down to 10 ns, corresponding to a ranging acuity of 1.7 microm. The current study was designed to measure the ranging acuity of the nectarivorous bat Glossophaga soricina under semi-natural conditions. Three free-flying bats were trained to discriminate between a stationary loudspeaker membrane and a membrane sinusoidally vibrating at 10 Hz. At detection threshold, the average peak-to-peak displacement of the vibrating membrane was 13 mm, corresponding to an echo delay jitter of 75 micros. The perceived jitter from call to call, which depends on the pulse interval and the call emission time relative to the membrane phase, was simulated for comparison with the virtual jitter experiments. This call-to-call jitter was between 20 to 25 micros (ca. 4 mm ranging acuity). These thresholds between 20 and 75 micros (4-13 mm) fall within both ecologically and physiologically plausible ranges, allowing for sufficiently precise navigation and foraging.
SUMMARYThe unique combination of flight and echolocation has opened the nocturnal air space as a rich ecological niche for bats. By analysing echoes of their sonar emissions, bats discriminate and recognize three-dimensional (3-D) objects. However, in contrast to vision, the 3-D information that can be gained by ensonifying an object from only one observation angle is sparse. To date, it is unclear how bats synchronize echolocation and flight activity to explore the 3-D shape of ensonified objects. We have devised an experimental design that allows creating 3-D virtual echo-acoustic objects by generating in real-time echoes from the batʼs emissions that depend on the batʼs position relative to the virtual object. Bats were trained to evaluate these 3-D virtual objects differing in their azimuthal variation of either echo amplitude or spectral composition. The data show that through a very effective coordination of sonar and flight activity, bats analyse an azimuthal variation of echo amplitude with a resolution of approximately 16dB and a variation of echo centre frequency of approximately 19%. Control experiments show that the bats can detect not only these variations but also perturbations in the spatial arrangement of these variations. The current experimental paradigm shows that echolocating bats assemble echo-acoustic object information -acquired sequentially in flight -to reconstruct the 3-D shape of the ensonified object. Unlike previous approaches, the recruitment of virtual objects allows for a direct quantification of this reconstruction success in a highly controlled experimental approach. Supplementary material available online at
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.