Research on people's confidence in their general knowledge has to date produced two fairly stable effects, many inconsistent results, and no comprehensive theory. We propose such a comprehensive framework, the theory of probabilistic mental models (PMM theory). The theory (a) explains both the overconfidence effect (mean confidence is higher than percentage of answers correct) and the hard-easy effect (overconfidence increases with item difficulty) reported in the literature and (b) predicts conditions under which both effects appear, disappear, or invert. In addition, (c) it predicts a new phenomenon, the confidence-frequency effect, a systematic difference between a judgment of confidence in a single event (i.e., that any given answer is correct) and a judgment of the frequency of correct answers in the long run. Two experiments are reported that support PMM theory by confirming these predictions, and several apparent anomalies reported in the literature are explained and integrated into the present framework.Do people think they know more than they really do? In the last 15 years, cognitive psychologists have amassed a large and apparently damning body of experimental evidence on overconfidence in knowledge, evidence that is in turn part of an even larger and more damning literature on so-called cognitive biases. The cognitive bias research claims that people are naturally prone to making mistakes in reasoning and memory, including the mistake of overestimating their knowledge. In this article, we propose a new theoretical model for confidence in knowledge based on the more charitable assumption that people are good judges of the reliability of their knowledge, provided that the knowledge is representatively sampled from a specified reference class. We claim that this model both predicts new experimental results (that we have tested) and explains a wide range of extant experimental findings on confidence, including some perplexing inconsistencies.Moreover, it is the first theoretical framework to integrate the two most striking and stable effects that have emerged from confidence studies-the overconfidence effect and the hardeasy effect-and to specify the conditions under which these effects can be made to appear, disappear, and even invert. In most recent studies (including our own, reported herein), subThis article was written while Gerd Gigerenzer was a fellow at the Center for Advanced Study in the Behavioral Sciences, Stanford, California. We are grateful for financial support provided by the Spencer Foundation and the Deutsche Foischungsgemeinschaft (DFG 170/2-1).We thank Leda Cosmides, Lorraine Daston, Baruch FischhofF, Jennifer Freyd, Kenneth Hammond, Wolfgang Hell, Sarah Licmenstein, Kathleen Much, John Tooby, Amos Tversky, and an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on earlier versions of this article.Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Gerd Gigerenzer, who is now at the Institut fur Psychologic, Hellbrunnerstrasse 34, Universitat Salzburg, A-5020 Salzburg, Austria.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.