Two experiments with 79 monolingual German speaking children between 2.5 and 4.5 years showed a consistent developmental gap between children's memory/inference of what someone wanted and what someone wrongly said or thought. For instance, when John is still playing and mother says, "John should be going to bed," more than 70% answered correctly that mother wanted John to go to bed. However, when mother said, "John is going to bed," about 70% answered wrongly that she thought/said that he was still playing. Correct answers emerged with the mastery of the false-belief task. In German, want sentences (about something to happen) obligatorily take the same grammatical that complement as say or think sentences. Therefore, the observed gap constrains de Villier's (1995) linguistic determinism, which claims that acquisition of the necessary grammatical structures for talking about the mind drives children's ability to think about the mind.
What do you know, if you know that a language has 'Object Verb' structure rather than 'Verb Object'? Answering this question and many others, this book provides an essential guide to the syntactic structure of German. It examines the systematic differences between German and English, which follow from this basic difference in sentence structure, and presents the main results of syntactic research on German. Topics covered include the strict word order in VO vs word order variation in OV, verb clustering, clause union effects, obligatory functional subject position, and subject-object asymmetries for extractions. Through this, a cross-model and cross-linguistic comparison evolves, highlighting the immediate implications for non-Germanic OV languages, and creating a detailed and comprehensive description of the syntactic differences that immediately follow from an OV type in contrast with a VO type like English. It will be of interest to all those interested in syntax and Germanic languages.
In this paper we argue for the following properties of clause-bound scrambling, as they are manifested in German. First, scrambling presupposes head-final projections. Only selected constituents, notably arguments, scramble, the reason being that phrases selected by a head have a unique base order. Second, scrambling involves antecedent-gap dependencies with A-chain properties. Third, scrambling is overt and non-string-vacuous. Fourth, scrambling is syntactically optional, clause-bound, category neutral, and may apply to more than one phrase per clause. Fifth, scrambled elements remain transparent for extraction; they are licit binders and take scope.Furthermore, we evaluate our conclusion that scrambling is contingent on the "OV" property by examining Yiddish, an uncontroversial scrambling Germanic language with controversial VO versus OV status. We argue that Yiddish is a variant of an OV language-thus allowing scrambling-and that it is the only Germanic language with alternative V-positions in a VP-shell structure, like Hindi, and, arguably, like Slavic languages. * 1 Chomsky (1995:324) suggests that operations such as extraposition and scrambling "may not really belong to the system we are discussing here as we keep closely to ... movement driven by feaure checking within the N-£ computation," where N is a numeration and £ is LF.206 Haider and Rosengren scrambling operates. In section 4 we derive the fact that scrambling applies to selected items only, notably arguments. Section 5 recapitulates the coverage of the facts under our analysis. In section 6 we argue that Yiddish is basically OV and not VO, whence scrambling is expected. Section 7 summarizes the paper. Theoretical Background.The main concern of this paper is an empirically adequate modeling of scrambling, paying attention to a nontechnical key issue of the minimalist program in its later versions (Chomsky 1998(Chomsky , 1999, namely optionality. Chomsky's (1998Chomsky's ( , 1999 ideas differ from those of Chomsky 1995 with respect to "bare output conditions": Covert movement is avoided and replaced by a matching operation that erases uninterpretable features of the target by matching them with features of the probe. As expected, these features are case and agreement features. Movement (or chain formation) is thus overt and feature-triggered. We argue that accounts in terms of feature-driven movement are inadequate for scrambling.In our view, overt operations are allowed when grammar does not forbid them and are licensed either by structural requirements, such as one that requires the movement of the finite verb in root V2-clauses, or by the possibility to exploit them in a systematic way at the syntaxexternal interfaces. This we assume to hold for the EPP as well as for scrambling. 3 Our starting point is a traditional CP-IP-VP structure for the Germanic VO languages. We do not see the need or justification for any inherently Agr-type projections. We assume only projections that may match the inflectional features of the verb, among others, TP. ...
We present a new analysis technique for EEG research on language comprehension, which dissociates superficially indistinguishable event-related potential (ERP) components. A frequency-based analysis differentiated between two apparently identical but functionally distinct N400 effects in terms of activity in separable frequency bands, and whether the activity stemmed from increased power or phase locking. Whereas linguistic problem detection is associated with theta band activity (approximately 3.5-7.5 Hz), conflict resolution correlates with activity in the delta band (1-3 Hz). The data further differentiate between the neuronal processing mechanisms involved in different types of conflict resolution on the basis of frequency characteristics (power vs phase locking).
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.