The most effective method for aggregating the conflicting opinions of experts is a subject of active debate in the literature. Task differences are most often used to explain differing results among studies. Alternatively, we suggested that the characteristics of the interacting groups themselves determine whether they outperform or underperform their equivalent composites. Expert loan officers serving in ad hoc and practiced groups, on average, performed equally as well as did their composite and most influential individual. However, whether a particular group outperformed or underperformed its composite could be explained by variation in group members' performances and abilities to recognize differential expertise. These findings suggest the circumstances in which alternative social decision schemes are likely to be more effective. They also support the usefulness of conceptualizing group judgment as a weighted combination of the opinions of group members whereby the allocation of weights to members is the critical issue.Individual versus group performance has been important in social psychology since the start of the century. The central issue in this research has been the extent to which the quality of group performance is above or below that of its members. More recently, however, there has been increased emphasis on the comparative performance of interacting groups and other alternative social decision schemes, particularly composite (or staticized) groups and best-member strategies. These analyses serve both a theoretical and an applied purpose. Comparisons of individual and group performance with these and other baseline models allow attribution of differences to a series of specific factors, increasing our understanding of group processes (e.g., Einhorn, Hogarth, & Klempner, 1977). They also provide a basis for selecting the most cost-effective decision scheme in different applied settings (e.g., Libby & Blashfield, 1978).The results of studies on the comparative performance of interacting and composite groups have been somewhat conflicting. Most studies examined mean performance on a particular task or compared mean performance across a series of tasks. The considerable variation in performance among participant groups was usually treated as experimental error. As a result, differences in task characteristics were most commonly used to explain conflicting results (e.g., see Hill, 1982). No previous study has examined differences across groups within a specific context (task) to determine the characteristics of particular in-We wish to thank Phil Yetton and Richard Hackman for their comments, and Jane Butt and Sarah Bonner for their assistance on this project.
Prior research has found support for contracting, political cost and information asymmetry explanations for managements' decision to revalue non-current assets. This study proposes that asset revaluations occur to signal available borrowing capacity via an increase in collateral values at the time of increases in secured debt and that the economic benefits associated with an asset revaluation will be greatest for firms when they are experiencing times of declining cash flows from operations. Results imply that firms that have undertaken an asset revaluation are more likely to be experiencing declining cash flows from operations than firms that have not revalued. This study also investigates whether the incidence of valuations coincides with increases in levels of secured borrowings due to lenders' demands for current values of assets offered as collateral. The evidence indicates that lirms are more likely to record an asset revaluation if they have increased their secured borrowings, and that most non-year-end revaluations emanate directly from contracting with lenders.In a recent issue of this journal, Brown, Izan and Loh (1992) and Whittred and Chan (1992) partially explain the occurrence of voluntary upward asset revaluations by Australian firms. Briefly, they find that revaluations are associated with the existence of debt contracts, high leverage, reduction of political costs, simultaneous issues of bonus shares, and avoidance of hostile takeover bids. Inevitably, these first two studies of such a complex issue leave a number of questions unanswered. For example, while Whittred and Chan's underinvestment argument partly explains the incidence of independent asset revaluations, it does not explain why firms undertake independent revaluations when they are not restricted by formal borrowing limitations; nor does it explain the large number of directors' valuations brought to account in published financial statements each year.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.