This is the unspecified version of the paper.This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link AbstractDeciding precisely when we have acted is challenging, as actions involve a train of neural events spread across both space and time. Repeated delays between actions and consequent events can result in a shift, such that immediate feedback can seem to precede the causative act. Here we examined which neurocognitive representations are affected during such sensorimotor temporal recalibration, by testing if the effect generalises across limbs, and whether it might reflect altered decision criteria for temporal judgements. Hand or foot adaptation phases were interspersed with simultaneity judgements about actions involving the same or opposite limb. Shifts in the distribution of participants' simultaneity responses were quantified using a detection-theoretic model, where a shift of both boundaries together gives a stronger indication that the effect is not simply a result of decision bias. By demonstrating that temporal recalibration occurs in the foot as well as the hand, we confirmed that it is a robust motor phenomenon:Both low and high boundaries shifted reliably in the same-limb conditions. However, in cross-limb conditions only the high boundary shifted reliably. These two patterns are interpreted to reflect a genuine change in how the time of action is represented, and a timing criterion shift, respectively.Keywords: Temporal recalibration, temporal judgement, adaptation, synchrony, action 4 Consider a falling apple. Our subjective experience of this well-known prompt for scientific insight is seamless and unified; the apple appears to fall as a coherent composite of colour, form and motion, before striking the ground with a dull thud. However, from the perspective of a homunculus (or indeed a neuroscientist) looking down upon the brain's activity in response to this sequence of events, the view is of a time-smeared and spatially distributed cacophony of electro-chemical signalling. This viewpoint leaves us wondering how the observer is able to decide with confidence that this particular auditory thud occurred at the same time as that particular visual collision.Science has not yet provided a compelling answer to this question. There is, however, evidence to suggest that recent experience plays an important role, or, put another way, that the temporal relationships that constitute simultaneity can be learnt and relearnt. Several methods exist to try and establish the relative time at which two events appear maximally synchronous (known as the point of subjective simultaneity or PSS). The most common methods require that participants report either the order of two events (a temporal order judgement; TOJ) or whether two or more events occurred synchronously or asynchronously (a simultaneity judgement; SJ). Intriguingly, when brief auditory and visual stimuli are repeatedly presented slightly out of synch during a period of adaptation, participants subsequen...
This is the unspecified version of the paper.This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link AbstractDeciding precisely when we have acted is challenging, as actions involve a train of neural events spread across both space and time. Repeated delays between actions and consequent events can result in a shift, such that immediate feedback can seem to precede the causative act. Here we examined which neurocognitive representations are affected during such sensorimotor temporal recalibration, by testing if the effect generalises across limbs, and whether it might reflect altered decision criteria for temporal judgements. Hand or foot adaptation phases were interspersed with simultaneity judgements about actions involving the same or opposite limb. Shifts in the distribution of participants' simultaneity responses were quantified using a detection-theoretic model, where a shift of both boundaries together gives a stronger indication that the effect is not simply a result of decision bias. By demonstrating that temporal recalibration occurs in the foot as well as the hand, we confirmed that it is a robust motor phenomenon:Both low and high boundaries shifted reliably in the same-limb conditions. However, in cross-limb conditions only the high boundary shifted reliably. These two patterns are interpreted to reflect a genuine change in how the time of action is represented, and a timing criterion shift, respectively.Keywords: Temporal recalibration, temporal judgement, adaptation, synchrony, action 4 Consider a falling apple. Our subjective experience of this well-known prompt for scientific insight is seamless and unified; the apple appears to fall as a coherent composite of colour, form and motion, before striking the ground with a dull thud. However, from the perspective of a homunculus (or indeed a neuroscientist) looking down upon the brain's activity in response to this sequence of events, the view is of a time-smeared and spatially distributed cacophony of electro-chemical signalling. This viewpoint leaves us wondering how the observer is able to decide with confidence that this particular auditory thud occurred at the same time as that particular visual collision.Science has not yet provided a compelling answer to this question. There is, however, evidence to suggest that recent experience plays an important role, or, put another way, that the temporal relationships that constitute simultaneity can be learnt and relearnt. Several methods exist to try and establish the relative time at which two events appear maximally synchronous (known as the point of subjective simultaneity or PSS). The most common methods require that participants report either the order of two events (a temporal order judgement; TOJ) or whether two or more events occurred synchronously or asynchronously (a simultaneity judgement; SJ). Intriguingly, when brief auditory and visual stimuli are repeatedly presented slightly out of synch during a period of adaptation, participants subsequen...
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.