In Experiments 1 and 2, rats were exposed to two compound flavors, AXand BX, containing one flavor in common (X). Following this exposure phase, an aversion was conditioned to A in the experimental group by pairing its consumption with an injection of lithium, while a control group drank A without being poisoned. The effect of this treatment was to establish B as a conditioned inhibitor. In Experiment 1, experimental animals were slower than controls to condition an aversion to B when its consumption was paired with lithium (a retardation test of conditioned inhibition). In Experiment 2, B alleviated the suppression of intake of another flavor previously paired with lithium (a summation test). Experiments 3 and 4 established that these effects depended upon prolonged prior exposure to AX and BX.In their theory of stimulus representation, McLaren, Kaye, and Mackintosh (1989) suggested that exposure to two compound stimuli (AX and BX), containing a common element X, would permit the establishment of two types of associations between the elements of each compound. The first would be excitatory associations between the common element X and each unique element, A and B. The existence of such associations is evident from studies such as that of Rescorla and Cunningham (1978), employing compound flavors. Their effect will presumably be to enhance generalization between the two compounds: if AX is paired with an unconditioned stimulus (US), the ability of X to retrieve a representation of B could result in excitatory conditioning to B as well as to A on this trial. Acquired equivalence effects of this sort have been demonstrated by Hall (1989, 1991).It is well established, however, that prolonged exposure to AX and BX will often facilitate subsequent discrimination, rather than increase generalization between them (Hall, 1991). McLaren et al. (1989) attribute this perceptual learning etTect, in part, to differences in the associability of the common (X) and unique (A and B) elements, which occur as a consequence of greater latent inhibition of the former than of the latter. Several studies have provided good evidence for such a suggestion (e.g., Mackintosh, Kaye, & Bennett, 1991; Rodrigo, Chamizo, McLaren, & Mackintosh, 1994) see Experiments 3 and 4) concluded that such differential latent inhibition of common and unique elements could not be the only explanation of perceptual learning. They suggested that one additional mechanism was provided by a second set of associations that are formed between the elements of AX and BX-~specifically, inhibitory associations between A and 8. The establishment of excitatory associations between common and unique elements may cause X to retrieve a representation of B on AX trials (and of A on BX trials), but the presence of A signals the absence of the otherwise expected B (just as the presence of B signals the absence of the otherwise expected A), and according to standard associative theory, this should lead to the eventual establishment of inhibitory associations between A and B...
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.