An evolutionary model for sex differences in disease risk posits that alleles conferring higher risk in one sex may be protective in the other. These sexually antagonistic (SA) alleles are predicted to be maintained at frequencies higher than expected under purifying selection against unconditionally deleterious alleles, but there are apparently no examples in humans. Discipline-specific terminology, rather than a genuine lack of such alleles, could explain this disparity. We undertook a two-stage review of evidence for SA polymorphisms in humans using search terms from (i) evolutionary biology and (ii) biomedicine. Although the first stage returned no eligible studies, the second revealed 51 genes with sex-opposite effects; 22 increased disease risk or severity in one sex but protected the other. Those with net positive effects occurred at higher frequencies. None were referred to as SA. Our review reveals significant communication barriers to fields as a result of discipline-specific terminology.
Sex differences in human disease risk and incidence are widely documented but their origins are poorly understood. An evolutionary model for differential disease risk between the sexes posits that alleles that are a risk factor (deleterious) in one sex may actually be protective (beneficial) in the opposite sex, and as such these sexually antagonistic (SA) alleles may be maintained in the population at frequencies higher than expected under purifying selection against unconditionally deleterious alleles. Sexual antagonism at genetic loci is thought to arise due to fundamental differences in how Darwinian fitness is maximized in the two sexes. Evidence for the occurrence of SA alleles comes from a number of non-human vertebrate and invertebrate models, which indicate they are common, although only very few specific SA loci have been identified. While there are many examples of loci with sex-limited effects in humans, there are no known examples of specific genetic variants that experience opposing patterns of selection between the sexes in humans. We hypothesized that this may be due to a mismatch in terminology between scientific disciplines rather than a genuine lack of examples. We therefore undertook a two-stage review of the literature for evidence of genetic polymorphisms in human disease and complex traits that exert their effects in a SA pattern, extracting effect sizes and allele frequencies where possible. In the first stage, we used search terms from evolutionary biology that aimed to identify examples of genes specifically referred to as having SA effects or were implicated in intralocus sexual conflict. In the second, we used terminology that was designed to capture as many examples as possible from the biomedical literature (e.g. sex-dependent, sex-opposite, men/women, boys/girls), which may have been missed in the first stage due to differences in terminology between the disciplines. The two searches gave very different results. The first stage returned 34 papers in total, none of which included evidence from humans of genetic loci with opposite effects on the two sexes. The second stage returned 881 papers, which after screening resulted in 70 papers, 32 were deemed eligible, and described 51 different genetic loci or haplotypes with sex-opposite effects on complex traits or disease. Of these, 22 loci increase disease risk or severity in one sex but are protective in the other. These loci may therefore be considered SA loci if the effects on disease risk translate into marginal effects on fitness through fecundity selection or mortality, although in no case were they referred to in terms of sexual antagonism. As predicted, alleles with net positive effects across the sexes occurred at a higher frequency than those with net negative effects. Our review reveals that significant long-term barriers to communication are occurring between scientific fields as a result of discipline-specific terminology. Less specific search terms revealed evidence that SA variants are maintained at relatively high frequencies in humans, apparently because while they contribute to the propensity for one sex to develop a range of diseases and disorders, they protect the other.
Sexual antagonism is thought to be an important selective force in multiple evolutionary processes, but very few examples of the genes involved are known. Such a deficit of loci could partially be explained by the lack of overlap in terminology between scientific disciplines. Following a similar review in humans, we searched systematically for studies that described genes with sexually antagonistic or sex‐opposite effects in any taxa, using terms designed to capture alternative descriptions of sexual antagonism. Despite drawing on a potentially very large pool of studies we found only eight articles, which between them described seven candidate variants, five of these were gene knockouts. In every case, the variants had net negative effects on the focal trait. One locus was independently validated between studies, but in comparison to previous data on variants in humans and the fruit‐fly, the studies generally suffered from small sample sizes, with concomitant high variance. Our review highlights the radically different effects that gene deletions can have on males and females, where the beneficial effects seen in one sex may facilitate the evolution of gene loss. We searched systematically for genetic variants with sexually antagonistic or sex‐opposite effects in any taxa. Of 2116 articles, we found seven candidate variants, five of which were gene knockouts. Our review highlights the radically different effects that gene deletions can have on males and females, where the beneficial effects seen in one sex may facilitate the evolution of gene loss.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.