The main purpose of the present study was to assess the test-retest reliability of a newly devised instrument for measuring optimism and pessimism. In addition, correlations were obtained between the optimism and pessimism scores and two measures of happiness as well as with subjects' responses to single-item questions about the likelihood of a nuclear encounter and about their religious and political commitment and philosophy oflife. The instruments were administered to 106 introductory psychology students on two occasions separated by a 2-week interval. The test-retest reliability for the optimism scale was .75; for pessimism, .84. Both optimism and pessimism were correlated, on both administrations, with both happiness measures in the high .50s to low .60s. Of the remaining items, the strongest relation was between optimism and religious commitment (r = .27), conflrming prior results.A newly devised instrument for measuring optimism and pessimism has been described by Dember, Martin, Hummer, Howe, and Melton (in press). The original impetus for that work stemmed from an interest in investigating the correlates of individual differences in susceptibility to the Pollyanna principle-a common tendency to "accentuate the positive" that is manifested in a wide variety of circumstances (see Matlin & Stang, 1978). Dember and Penwell (1980) reported modest correlations between one measure of the Pollyanna tendency and two indices of subjective well-being, or happiness. The measure of happiness used was chosen by virtue of its ease of administration and its good psychometric properties (Kammann, Christie, Irwin, & Dixon, 1979); a direct measure of optimism would have been preferable, but an acceptable one was not available at the time.The Dember et al. instrument was devised speciflcally to flll that gap. In the meantime, interest in optimism and pessimism burgeoned, stimulated in large part by the revised learned helplessness model of Seligman and his collaborators (e.g., Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978).Research performed within that framework has identified what have been called optimistic and pessimistic explanatory styles as implicated in physical health (Kamen, Rodin, & Seligman, 1987; Peterson, Seligman, & Vaillant, 1988) and even in the attractiveness of political candidates (Zullow, Oettingen, Peterson, & Seligman, 1988). That work was carried out with the aid of an instrument for measuring explanatory style, the Attributional Style Questionnaire (ASQ; see Peterson et al., 1982), or with a content analysis system (Content Analysis of Verbatim Explanations, or CAVE; see Peterson, Luborsky, & Seligman, 1983) based on the ASQ for use with existing documents.Correspondence may be addressed to William N. Dember, Department of Psychology, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH 45221-0376.
365At present, then, there are competing instruments for measuring optimism and pessimism, including a brief scale developed by Cloninger (1987) and one, very similar to ours, by Scheier and Carver (1985). It would be ...