When 2 cues occur together and reliably predict an outcome, Ss often judge the effect of the compound as reducible to the individual effects of the elements. This elemental processing in predictive learning is perhaps the single most important aspect of most theories of human inference. Surprisingly, selectional processing was not observed in either blocking or conditioned inhibition problems. Only when the learner had past experience with another problem encouraging an elemental strategy were the expected selectional processes observed. These proactive effects of prior learning were abolished if the earlier problem required a nonadditive solution. The results suggest that configural cues were guiding predictive inferences in the absence of elemental processes.Many authors have been struck by the formal similarity of the problem of covariation detection in humans and animals (e.g., Gluck & Bower, 1988;Holyoak, Koh, & Nisbett, 1989;Shanks & Dickinson, 1987). Both organisms must learn to detect regularities in their environment and to take appropriate actions. That is, predictive signals must be identified in a stream of irrelevant cues, whether these signals predict the romantic interest of another person or patches of food for a hungry animal. These parallels raise the intriguing possibility that similar learning mechanisms may explain how humans and animals solve formally analogous problems.This possibility is encouraged when variables in the human and animal domains have corresponding effects. For example, in assessing the contingency between an action and an outcome, people are sensitive to the likelihood of the outcome in the presence versus absence of the action (e.g., Chatlosh, Neunaber, & Wasserman, 1985). The outcome is presumed to be contingent only if the action influences the background rate of occurrence of the outcome. By no means are the reported contingency effects equivalent to statistical measures of covariation. People often overestimate zero contingencies, especially if there is a high probability of the outcome (Alloy & Tabachnik, 1984). Animals show a similar sensitivity to contingency (Rescorla, 1968) and also respond inappropriately early in training when there is a nominal zero contingency (e.g., Benedict & Ayres, 1972). Shared imperfections in covariation detection like these can be explained by an associative model (e.g., Wasserman, Elek, Chatlosh, & Baker, 1993).Although there has been considerable progress in our understanding of the cognitive processes underlying predictive
This study investigated, in a laboratory setting, whether prospective memory (remembering to perform intended actions in the future) would be improved by self-enactment of the to-be-remembered tasks. The subjects, 45 university students, were asked to remember later to perform five tasks that they initially enacted themselves, watched the experimenter perform, or had described to them. These tasks were to be performed, ostensibly in preparation for the next subject, at the end of 30 min of filler activity, which was presented as the experimental task. Surprisingly, self-enactment produced the poorest prospective remembering. Speculative explanations are offered in terms of both metacognitive expectations about memory and output-monitoring deficiencies.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.