1. Single-unit activity was recorded from the superior colliculus (SC) of monkeys trained to look to visual targets presented on an oscilloscope screen. On one task, target localization required that information concerning the retinal position of the target be combined with information concerning current or future eye position. This task also permitted a dissociation between the site of retinal stimulation and the metrics of the saccade triggered by the stimulation. 2. Vigorous visual responses of superficial SC neurons may occur that do not result in the activation of underlying saccade-related cells. The activity of these neurons signals the occurrence of a visual stimulus, whether or not the stimulus is selected for foveal viewing. 3. Saccade-related (SR) discharges of most intermediate and deep-layer SC neurons precede saccades with particular vectors, regardless of the region of retinal activation initiating the saccade. The discharge of these neurons is tightly coupled to saccade onset, even if changes in eye position have occurred since target appearance. Thus, the discharge of these SR neurons must occur after retinal error and eye-position signals have been combined to compute the necessary saccade vector. For most SR neurons, direct retinal activation of overlying visual neurons had no effect on either the vigor or probability of a SR discharge. The discharge of overlying visual cells is neither necessary nor sufficient to activate most SR cells. 4. The discharge of some SR cells is dependent on prior activation of overlying visual cells. Of 53 SR cells, only 3 were completely dependent on visual stimulation, while another 8 discharged less vigorously if corresponding visual activation failed to occur. 5. About one-quarter of the SR cells showed long-lead preburst activity. This activation was characterized by a low level of firing, which began after the saccade signal and continued until a saccade-linked burst occurred. 6. Cells were isolated that were visually responsive yet discharged prior to saccades in the absence of appropriate retinal stimulation. No component of the discharge of these quasi-visual (QV) cells appeared to be motor in the usual sense. The activity of these neurons appears to reflect eye-position error (the difference between actual and desired eye position) and to hold this information in spatial register until a saccade occurs or is cancelled. 7. It is concluded that the presumed linkage, implied in earlier versions of the foveation hypothesis, between the superficial layers (receiving direct retinal inputs) and the deeper layers of the SC is not necessary for the activation of SR neurons. Results suggest that the SC must generate or receive a signal that combines retinal error and eye-position information. These findings are discussed in terms of current models of the saccadic-control system.
Single-unit recordings were made from midbrain areas in monkeys trained to make both conjugate and disjunctive (vergence) eye movements. Previous work had identified cells with a firing rate proportional to the vergence angle, without regard to the direction of conjugate gaze. The present study describes the activity of neurons that burst for disjunctive eye movements. Convergence burst cells display a discrete burst of activity just before and during convergence eye movements. For most of these cells, the profile of the burst is correlated with instantaneous vergence velocity and the number of spikes in the burst is correlated with the size of the vergence movement. Some of these cells also have a tonic firing rate that is positively correlated with vergence angle (convergence burst-tonic cells). Divergence burst cells have similar properties, except that they fire for divergent and not convergent movements. Divergence burst cells are encountered far less often than convergence burst cells. Both convergence and divergence burst cells were found in an area of the mesencephalic reticular formation just dorsal and lateral to the oculomotor nucleus. Convergence burst cells were also recorded in another more dorsal mesencephalic region, rostral to the superior colliculus. Both of the areas also contain cells that encode vergence angle. Models of the vergence system derived from psychophysical data imply the existence of a vergence integrator, the output of which is vergence angle. Some models also suggest the presence of a parallel element that improves the frequency response of the vergence system, but has no effect on the steady-state behavior of the system. Vergence burst cells would be suitable inputs to a vergence integrator. By providing a vergence velocity signal to motoneurons, they may improve the dynamic response of the vergence system. The behavior of vergence burst cells during vergence movements is similar to that of the medium-lead burst cells during saccades. The proposed roles for vergence velocity cells are analogous to those of the saccadic burst cells. In this respect, the neural organization of the vergence system resembles that of the saccadic system, despite the distinct difference in the kinematics of these two types of eye movements.
Animals with binocular single vision use disjunctive (vergence) eye movements to align the two eyes on a visual target. Several lines of evidence suggest that conjugate and vergence eye movement commands are generated independently and combined at the medial rectus motoneurons. If this were true, then a pure vergence eye-position signal should exist. This signal would be proportional to the horizontal angle between the eyes (vergence angle), without regard to the direction of conjugate gaze. The purpose of this experiment was to identify and study neurons that carry a pure vergence signal. Extracellular unit recordings were made from midbrain and pontine sites in monkeys trained to track visual targets moving in the horizontal, vertical, and depth (or target vergence) planes. The most commonly encountered neuron that had a vergence signal was the convergence cell. These units had a firing rate that was linearly proportional to the convergence angle; their activity was unaffected by changes in conjugate gaze. Changes in convergence cell activity preceded the change in vergence angle slightly. Convergence cell activity increased for increased convergence regardless of whether the change was in response to purely accommodative or disparity cues. Divergence cells were found far less frequently. These cells were similar to convergence cells except that they decreased their firing rate for increases in convergence. The activity of divergence cells was unaffected by changes in the direction of conjugate gaze. Both convergence and divergence cells were found, intermixed, in the mesencephalic reticular formation must outside the oculomotor nucleus. Most cells with a vergence signal were found within 1-2 mm of the nucleus. These results support the view that conjugate and vergence signals are generated independently and are combined at the extraocular motoneurons. Convergence cells seem ideally suited to provide the vergence signal required by the nearby medial rectus motoneurons.
Chronic unit recording experiments conducted over the past two decades have identified many functional classes of neurons with saccade-related activity that reside in a host of brainstem nuclei. Older models of the saccadic system were based upon the properties of only a few of these functional types of neurons. They described the putative flow of signals through the brainstem circuitry and specified some, but not all, of the signal transformations to be performed. How the necessary computations were performed by neurons was not always explicit. Recent experiments investigating the neural control of saccadic eye movements and modifications of the original models are designed to fill in the details of the broad sketch of saccadic circuitry originally available. This review suggests one strategy for proceeding with this effort. Saccadic command signals observed in the SC require transformation to interface with the burst generators and motoneuron pools innervating the extraocular muscles. Specifying the signal transformations required for this interface should facilitate the design of experiments directed toward an understanding of the functional properties of cells located in nuclei intervening between the SC and the pulse/step circuitry, subsets of neurons that often have no role in models of the saccadic system. In this review, we hypothesize that neurons residing in various tectorecipient brainstem nuclei participate in one or more of the required signal transformations. The pathway from SC to cMRF and PPRF may be involved in the extraction of information about the amplitude and/or velocity of the horizontal component of oblique saccades. The pathway from SC to NRTP and cerebellar vermis may act selectively to generate signals compensating for the presaccadic orbital position. Finally, the activity of LLBNs and MLBs discharging maximally before oblique saccades may form the basis of computations required to match component velocity with overall saccade direction and amplitude. Although the data supporting these speculations are meager at present, such conjectures do form the basis of working hypotheses that can be tested experimentally. We also considered the implications of kinematic constraints, especially Donders' and Listing's laws, for future investigations. Tweed & Vilis (1987, 1990) proposed models specifically designed to handle these constraints. In their models, eye position is represented on four oculomotor channels: three coding the vector components of eye position, and one carrying a signal inversely related to gaze eccentricity and torsion. Yet, other evidence suggest that simpler computations may suffice for the implementation of laws that are only approximately obeyed.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.