A widespread view concerning financial decision-making is that women are more riskaverse than men. A consequence of this stereotype is statistical discrimination which diminishes the success of women in financial and labor markets. The perception that female managers are less risk-prone than men has been put forward as a major cause of ''glass ceilings'' in corporate promotion ladders (Johnnie E. V. Johnson and Philip L. Powell, 1994). Women are less trusted than men to make the risky decisions that may be necessary for a firm's success. Anecdotal evidence from financial markets suggests that similar stereotyping by investment brokers is possibly to the disadvantage of female clients. Women are expected to be more conservative investors than men and are consequently offered investments with lower risks and therefore lower expected returns (Penelope Wang, 1994).Given that preconceptions concerning the risk propensities of men and women seem to affect their economic success, it is important to examine whether the above stereotype reflects actual economic behavior. There is some evidence supporting the view that women are more risk-averse than men in financial decision-making. Recent survey data suggest that wealth holdings of single women are less risky than those of single men of equal economic status (, 1998). Also, when asked about their attitudes toward financial risks, women seem to report a lower risk propensity than men (Robert B. Barsky et al., 1997). Finally, experimental evidence suggests that women may be more risk-averse than men toward gambles (Irwin P. Levin et al., 1988). It is questionable, however, whether stereotypic risk attitudes can be confirmed by the above survey and experimental evidence. First, in survey data, gender-specific risk attitudes may be confounded with differences in individual opportunity sets. Survey studies of individual wealth composition provide only weak control for gender-specific information on choice options and gender-specific wealth constraints in the underlying financial decisions.Second, behavior in abstract gambling experiments may not correspond to risk behavior in contextual decisions, as has been shown by John Hershey and Paul J. H. Schoemaker (1980). Thus, while abstract gambling experiments provide strong control of the economic environment surrounding risky decisions, their data may not be adequate for drawing conclusions on gender-specific risk attitudes of investors and managers.The objective of the study reported here is to circumvent the above problems in analyzing gender-specific risk attitudes. We conducted an experiment in which subjects not only made abstract gambling decisions, but were also confronted with financially motivated risky decisions embedded in an investment or insurance context. The implementation of such contextual decisions allowed us to examine gender-specific risk propensities in contexts which may be relevant to financial and labor markets. At the same time, compared to survey 382 AEA PAPERS AND PROCEEDINGS MAY 1999TABLE 1-EXPERIMENT...
Purpose -The purpose of this paper is to describe how to measure workplace configuration, show its application in, and the results of, a field experiment aimed at improving collaborative knowledge work and identify and discuss larger problems involved with research on workplace configuration. Design/methodology/approach -The paper reviews recent thought on density, proximity and problems with evaluating configuration. It then describes a method of spatial network analysis used in a field experiment involving the reconfiguration of a workplace. This is followed with a discussion of recent research on knowledge work from economic geography. Findings -It was found that, instead of increasing it, the reconfigured workplace decreased collaborative activity. The spatial network analysis shows how this occurred. Research limitations/implications -This method of spatial network analysis, when used carefully, is a robust technique for analyzing and comparing spatial configuration. Further research needs to address the links between spatial proximity and information and communications technologies as well as the relation of types of knowledge bases and associated forms of proximity that can stimulate collaboration. Practical implications -While spatial network analysis methods are not do-it-yourself tools, corporate real estate managers should employ them, especially in larger-scale projects, before committing to final workplace designs. They can also use them to identify and map best spatial patterns (like best practices) to identify strategic spatial patterns. Originality/value -This appears to be the first rigorous application of spatial network methods in a field experiment involving a real workplace. The paper shows the method can clearly extract and discriminate spatial network patterns underlying configurations and relate them both quantitatively and graphically to employee evaluations of collaborative performance. It introduces concepts of comparative knowledge bases that need to be understood in determining the types of collaboration needed in a workplace.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.