MR images accurately depict normal patterns of age-related change in intracranial space, whole brain, GM, WM, and CSF. These quantitative MR imaging data can be used in research studies and clinical settings for the detection of abnormalities in fundamental neuroanatomic parameters.
Despite the many models of saccadic eye movements, little attention has been paid to the shape of saccade trajectories. Some investigators have argued that saccades are driven by a rectangular "bang-bang" neural control signal, whereas others have emphasized the similarity to fast arm movement trajectories, such as the "minimum jerk" profile. However, models have not been tested rigorously against empirical trajectories. We examined the Fourier transforms of saccades and compared them with theoretical models. Horizontal saccades were recorded from 10 healthy subjects. The Fourier transform of each saccade was accurately computed using a padded fast Fourier transform (FFT), and the frequencies of the first three minima (M1, M2, M3) in each energy spectrum were measured to a precision of 0.12 Hz. Each subject showed near-linear trends in the relationships among M1, M2, and M3 and the reciprocal of duration (1/T), which we call the "spectral main sequence." Extrapolation of plots did not pass through the origin, indicating a subtle departure from self-similarity. Bivariate confidence regions were established to allow for slope-intercept variability. The nonharmonic relationships seen cannot arise from a rectangular saccadic pulse driving a linear ocular plant. The relationships are also incompatible with minimum acceleration, minimum jerk, or higher-order minimum square derivative trajectories. The best fits were made by trajectories that minimize postmovement variance with signal-dependent noise (). It is concluded that the spectral main sequence is exquisitely sensitive to the saccade trajectory and should be used to test objectively all present and future models of saccades.
Harwood MR, Madelain L, Krauzlis RJ, Wallman J. The spatial scale of attention strongly modulates saccade latencies. J Neurophysiol 99: 1743-1757, 2008. First published January 30, 2008 doi:10.1152/jn.00589.2007. We have previously shown that when a stimulus consisting of two concentric rings moves, saccade latencies are much longer (by 150 ms) when attention is directed to the larger ring than to the smaller ring. Here, we investigated whether this effect can be explained by a deferral of the "cost" of making a saccade while the target remains inside the attentional field, or by purely visual factors (eccentricity or contrast). We found 1) latencies were shorter when attention was directed to small features irrespective of retinal eccentricity; 2) saccade latency distributions were systematically determined by the ratio between the amplitude of the stimulus step and the diameter of the attended ring: stimulus steps that were larger than the attended ring resulted in short latencies, whereas steps smaller than the attended ring resulted in proportionally longer and more variable latencies; 3) this effect was not seen in manual reaction times to the same target movement; and 4) suprathreshold changes in the contrast of targets, mimicking possible attentional effects on perceived contrast and saliency, had little effect on latency. We argue that the spatial scale of attention determines the urgency of saccade motor preparation processes by changing the rate and rate variability of the underlying decision signal, to defer the cost of saccades that result in little visual benefit. I N T R O D U C T I O NThe classic experimental situation for studying saccades is to have a person or monkey fixating a target, which unexpectedly jumps to a new location, thereby eliciting a saccade. The latency of the resulting saccade has been much studied both as an exemplar of simple decisions and in terms of the temporal processes that make up the latency. With respect to the former, the latency distribution has been modeled either as a randomwalk or diffusion process in which the decision variable is subject to noise at each moment (Ratcliff 1978) or as a process in which the noise is manifested by trial-to-trial changes in the rate of accumulation of information indicating that the target has moved ["LATER" (Linear Approach to Threshold with Ergodic Rate) model; Carpenter and Williams 1995]. These models have been reviewed by Glimcher (2003), Schall (2003, and Smith and Ratcliff (2004). With respect to the components of the saccadic latency, in addition to the influence of purely visual characteristics of the target and the constraints imposed by the time required to program and execute a saccade, attention can shorten the saccade latency either by being drawn to the target location by a cue or by being withdrawn from the previous location by having a gap before the target appears (Posner 1980;Weber and Fischer 1995).Implicit in most latency models is the notion that, because vision is most acute at the fovea, it is desirable to ...
Objectives: To measure vertical and horizontal responses to optokinetic (OK) stimulation and investigate directional abnormalities of quick phases in progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP). Methods: Saccades and OK nystagmus were studied in six PSP patients, five with Parkinson's disease (PD), and 10 controls. The OK stimulus subtended 72˚horizontally, 60˚vertically, consisted of black and white stripes, and moved at 10-50˚/s. Results: All PSP patients showed slowed voluntary vertical saccades and nystagmus quick phases compared with PD or controls. Small, paired, horizontal saccadic intrusions (SWJ) were more frequent and larger in PSP during fixation. Vertical saccades were transiently faster at the time of SWJ and horizontal saccades in PSP. During vertical OK nystagmus, small quick phases were often combined with horizontal SWJ in all subjects; in PSP the vector was closer to horizontal. Vertical OK slow phase gain was reduced in PSP but, in most PD patients, was similar to normals. The average position of gaze shifted in the direction of vertical OK stimulus in PSP patients with preserved slow phase responses but impaired quick phases. Conclusions: Vertical OK responses in PSP show impaired slow phase responses, and quick phases that are slowed and combined with SWJ to produce an oblique vector. SWJ facilitate vertical saccades and quick phases in PSP, but it is unclear whether this is an adaptive process or a result of the disease. A large OK stimulus is useful to induce responses that can be quantitatively analysed in patients with limited voluntary range of vertical gaze.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.