Link to this article: http://journals.cambridge.org/abstract_S0022226714000024How to cite this article: AKIKO NAGANO and MASAHARU SHIMADA (2014). Morphological theory and orthography: Kanji as a representation of lexemes .Orthography has been given marginal status in theoretical linguistics, but it can offer 'visible' insights into the invisible mechanisms of grammar. Japanese kanji graphs, Chinese characters used to write Japanese, provide an excellent illustration of this perspective. Our core claim is that the kanji orthography reflects the working of lexeme-based morphology in Japanese grammar. Specifically, we show how the lexeme-based morphological framework developed by Mark Aronoff and Martin Maiden can explain apparently cumbersome and inefficient properties of the kanji usage, its dual pronunciation in particular. Among the findings of this study are the following: (i) the underlying mechanism of the kanji's dual pronunciation is suppletion, native and Sino-Japanese synonyms working as morphomic stems of the same paradigm; (ii) this suppletion emerged and developed as a paradigmatic strategy of synonymy avoidance; and (iii) the large-scale suppletive morphology has long been retained in Japanese because it has served advantageous functions in the maintenance of lexemic isomorphism and in lexical stock expansion. Our findings shed an entirely new light on the bafflingly complex nature of Japanese orthography; it is the complexity of morphology, a grammatical module that is deemed to be the locus of language-specificity.[] The original manuscript of this paper has been drastically revised following three anonymous Journal of Linguistics referees' comments and suggestions. We would like to express our deep gratitude to each of them for carefully reading our manuscript and generously providing us with ideas and data for improvement. Also, we are grateful for the comments from the participants of the ICHL workshop 'The Role of Autonomous Morphology in Language Change' (organised by Martin Maiden and John Charles Smith, July , Osaka), where the initial idea of this paper was presented. Our special thanks go to Ewa Jaworska for invaluable editorial support. Needless to say, we are solely responsible for the contents of this paper. This study is financially supported by the Japan Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology (Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research (C), No. ). Abbreviations used in this paper are: = second person, A = adjective, ABL = ablative, ACC = accusative, Adv = adverbial, AGT = agent, C = consonant, Comp = compound, CONCL = conclusive, GEN = genitive, HON = honorific, IMP = imperative, Infl = inflection, INF = infinitive, L = lexeme, NOM = nominative, P = stem, P Adv = adverbial stem, P Comp = compounding stem, P Infl = inflectional stem, PL = plural, PRT = particle, SJ = Sino-Japanese, SG = singular, V= vowel, VOC = vocative, WFA = word-forming affix.
In this paper, we attempt to determine the underlying word order of Old English (OE) including the position of INFL. We will show that Early OE and Late OE are SOVI (S-VP-I) and SIOV (S-I-VP) languages, respectively, pointing out a striking difference in the distribution of stressless pronouns between them. On the basis of the underlying word order change in the history of English, we will propose a principle regulating word order change, which we call here the Principle of Parameter Percolation (PPP).* * This paper is a slightly revised version of a paper read at the Seventh General Meeting of the Modern English Association held at Shujitsu Women's University on May 18, 1990. We are grateful to Satoshi Ohta and two anonymous EL reviewers for their helpful comments and suggestions. Thanks also go to Roger Martin for suggesting stylistic improvements. All remaining errors, needless to say, are ours.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.