An audiovisual stimulus was made contingent upon the rat's licking at the water spout, thus making it analogous with a gustatory stimulus. When the audiovisual stimulus and the gustatory stimulus were paired with electric shock the avoidance reactions transferred to the audiovisual stimulus, but not the gustatory stimulus. Conversely, when both stimuli were paired with toxin or x-ray the avoidance reactions transferred to the gustatory stimulus, but not the audiovisual stimulus. Apparently stimuli are selected as cues dependent upon the nature of the subsequent reinforcer.A great deal of evidence stemming from diverse sources suggests an inadequacy in the usual formulations concerning reinforcement. Barnett (1963) has described the "bait-shy" behavior of wild rats which have survived a poisoning attempt. These animals utilizing olfactory and gustatory cues. avoid the poison bait which previously made them ill. However. there is no evidence that they a void the "place" of the poisoning.In a recent volume (Haley & Snyder. 1964) several authors have discussed studies in which ionizing radiations were employed as a noxious stimulus to produce avoidance reactions in animals. Ionizing radiation like many poisons produces gastrointestinal disturbances and nausea. Strong aversions are readily established in animals when distinctively flavored fluids are conditionally paired with x-rays. Subsequently. the gustatory stimulus will depress fluid intake without radiation. In contrast. a distinctive environmental complex of auditory. visual, and tactual stimUli does not inhibit drinking even when the compound stimulus is associated with the identical radiation schedule. This differential effect has also been observed following ingestion of a toxin and the injection of a drug (Garcia & Koelling. 1965).Apparently this differential effectiveness of cues is due either to the nature of the reinforcer. i.e .• radiation or toxic effects. or to the peculiar relation which a gustatory stimulus has to the drinking response. ·i.e .• gustatory stimulation occurs if and only if the animal licks the fluid. The environmental cues associated with a distinctive place are not as dependent upon a single response of the organism. Therefore. we made an auditory and visual stimulus dependent upon the animal's licking the water spout. Thus. in four experiments reported here "bright-noisy" water. as well as "tasty" water was conditionally paired with radiation. a toxin. immediate shock, and delayed shock. respectively. as reinforcers. Later the capacity of these responsecontrolled stimUli to inhibit drinking in the absence of reinforcement was tested.Psychon. Sci., 1966, Vol. <1 MethodThe apparatus was a light and sound shielded box (7 in. x 7 in. x in.) with a drinking spout connected to an electronic drinkometer which counted each touch of the rat's tongue to the spout. "Brightnoisy" water was provided by connecting an incandescent lamp (5 watts) and a clicking relay into this circuit. "Tasty" water was provided by adding flavors to the drinking s...
HARVARD MEDICAL SCHOOL AND NEUROSURGICAL SERVICE, MASSACHUSETTS GENERAL HOSPITALGustatory aversions, induced in rats by conditionally pairinfl a distinctive flavor with a noxious drug, were readily established wen when injections were delayed an hour or more. The optimal interstimulus interval and effectiveness of cues for learning appear to be a function of the specific effects of the reinforcer on the organism.It is considered axiomatic in theory and practice that no learning will occur without immediate reinforcement. For example, a hungry rat will not learn to press a lever for food unless the response is immediately followed by food (primary reinforcement) or by a signal which has been associated with food in the past (secondary reinforcement). Food can be described as rewarding, but the same general rule has been applied to punishing agents also. Delays of the order of 3 to 45 sec. have a deleterious effect upon learning in a wide variety of experimental situations. The significance of these findings for reinforcement theory was discussed by Spence (1947) and a recent review (Renner, 1964) reveals there has been no major modification of the temporal contiguity aspect. However, our data indicates that immediate reinforcement is not a general requirement for all learning. MethodYoung adult male rats (Sprague-Dawley, 300 to 400 gm)were maintained in individual cages with Purina Laboratory Chow ad lib. Drinking was restricted to a 10-min, period each day. After one week of habituation to this schedule, treatment began. In Experiment A, five groups (N = 8 each) were treated. One experimental group (Sac-Apo:inj) was given a gustatory cue in its drinking water (1 gm saccharine per liter) and after a delay was injected with a drug which produced gastriC disturbances (7mg/kg apomorphine hydrochloride I.P.). The animals were injected in serial order at I-min. intervals with the first animal injected at 5 min. and the last one at 12 min. after the saccharin water bottle was removed from the home cage. One control group (Sac-Sal:inj) drank saccharinwater and was injected with saline, while another control (Wat-Apo:inj) drank water and was injected with apomorphine. An additional experimental group (SacApo:inj) received delayed injections in serial order from 15 to 22 min. post-drinking. Other rats (Sac-Shock), immediately after drinking saccharin -water, were taken from their cages and placed in a box with an electric grid floor and three shocks (0.5 sec. pulses at 3 mal were delivered within 1 min. to the paws.All groups received four treatments, one every third day and then three extinction tests (Le. no injections or shock) on the same schedule. Between treatment days, the animals were given water for 10 min.
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2024 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.