Investigations studying fear in the rat have reported that a CS paired in a backward relationship with the US can acquire excitatory properties. This procedure does not appear to be effective in rabbit eyelid conditioning. The present studies examined Rescorla's proposal that an excitatory effect might be observed with US-CS pairings in eyelid conditioning if the CS was tested in a second-order conditioning paradigm. The results provided no support for this proposal because second-order eyelid conditioning was just as difficult to demonstrate as was backward eyelid conditioning.According to Pavlov (1927Pavlov ( /1960, the conditioned stimulus (CS) should begin to operate before the unconditioned stimulus (US) comes into action, if conditioning is to occur. With rabbit eyelid conditioning, the evidence indicates that backward pairings do not result in conditioned excitation (plotkin & Oakley, 1975;Siegel & Domjan, 1971; Smith, Coleman, & Gormezano, 1969; Holmes, Note 1). Examples in the conditioned suppression literature, however, demonstrate that US.cS pairings can result in conditioning (Heth, 1976;Heth & Rescorla, 1973;Keith-Lucas & Guttman, 1975;Mahoney & Ayres, 1976).Rescorla (Note 2) has suggested that the lack of overt responding after backward pairings in the eyelid preparation does not necessarily imply that the subject knows nothing about the relationship between the CS and the US. He stated (Rescorla, 1976) that "consequences of first-order conditioning which are not observed in the response to the first-order CS may nevertheless be observed in its ability to establish conditioning to higher order stimuli." Thus, he suggests that second-order conditioning may provide a more sensitive measure of what is learned from US-CS pairings. EXPERIMENT 1In this experiment, we examined the effects of US-CS pairings in rabbit eyelid conditioning. In addition to traditional measures of conditioning, we employed the second-order conditioning test suggested by Rescorla.This report is based on a thesis submitted to the Graduate School of Northwestern University by the nrst author in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the MS degree. It was supported by NSF Grant BMS 75-02313, awarded to the second author. Requests for reprints should be directed to Steven Stern, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, 2021 Sheridan Road, Evanston, Illinois 60201. MethodSubjects. The subjects were 70 male rabbits, weighing approximately 1.8 kg. Animals were housed individually and had free access to water and Purina Rabbit Chow. The colony was maintained at a temperature of 21° ± 2°C and 50% relative humidity (± 15%). Automated overhead lights were on from 7:00 a.m. to 8:00 p.m. daily.Apparatus. The rabbits were conditioned in a large, soundattenuated, lightproofwaIk-in chamber. Temperature and relative humidity were kept at 20° ± 1°C and 50% ± 10%, respectively. When in the chamber, each rabbit wore a small Plexiglas platform to which was attached a 6-Y bulb and photocell, positioned on an aluminum arm, approximatel...
Second-order conditioning has been frequently observed with the fear response but not with the eyelid response. The present experiments manipulated the temporal relationship between the second-order and first-order stimulus on second-order conditioning trials. Our results indicated that a trace second-order procedure is not effective with either response system. Second-order fear conditioning was most prominent when the second-order CS terminated at the onset of the first-order CS. This arrangement, however, did not produce second-order eyelid CRs. In eyelid conditioning, the second-order CS appears to inhibit responding to the first-order CS which immediately followsit.Pavlov (1927, p. 33) reported that a response originally conditioned to one stimulus could be conditioned to another when the two were paired in the absence of the US. Investigations employing the conditioned fear response have demonstrated reliable second-order conditioning. McAllister and McAllister (1964) trained rats to hurdle-jump to avoid shock. A group which received forward pairings of the second-order and first-order stimulus (CS z and CS.. respectively) showed consistent secondorder responding, whereas a backward conditioning control group did not. Other control procedures, such as noncontingent presentations of either CSt and the US (Rizley & Rescorla, 1972, Experiment 1) or CS z and CSt (Kamil, 1968), have also been examined. These studies demonstrate that both CSt and the US, as well as CS z and CS.. must be paired in order for responding to CS z to develop. These outcomes clearly establish second-order fear conditioning as a bona fide associative phenomenon.In contrast, investigations of second-order con- second-order fear conditioning with an analogous set of procedures.The difficulty in obtaining second-order .eyelid conditioning may be due to the use of an inappropriate time relationship between CS z and CSt. This hypothesis is based on Pavlov's (1927, p. 33) specification that for second-order conditioning to be effective, a temporal gap must separate CS z and CSt; i.e., "the essential condition is that the new stimulus should be withdrawn some seconds before the primary stimulus is applied." Fear conditioning data have not supported this notion. For example, Maisiak and Frey (1977, Experiment 1) found that second-order fear conditioning was less probable when a gap separated CS z and CSt than when the onset of CSt coincided with the termination of CS z. Similarly, Rescorla (1973) found that a delay secondorder fear conditioning procedure was superior to a trace procedure. The present experiments provide a more detailed analysis of Pavlov's hypothesis by systematically manipulating the temporal relationship of CS z and CSt in second-order conditioning. EXPERIMENT 1 MethodSUbjects. The subjects were 48 male albino rabbits ranging from IS to 20 weeks in age and weighing approximately 1.8 kg. The animals were housed individually with free access to water and Purina Rabbit Chow. Four animals died of respiratory infections, and the...
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2025 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.