The relation between speech perception and reading ability was investigated. Identification and discrimination tests with stimuli on a stop consonant place of articulation continuum (/pA/-/tA/) were presented to various groups of good and poor readers (normal first graders on a reading continuum in Study 1, moderately and severely dyslexic and high-and low-achieving normal children in Study 2). In both studies, reading-related perception differences were especially marked in a comparison of actual and predicted (from identification test) discrimination scores, suggesting that the poor readers have special difficulty with a discrimination-specific task demand. In Study 3, reading-group differences still show up if the stimulus pairs of the discrimination test are simply to be repeated (eliminating the similarity judgment factors), and are also very pronounced on a nonspeech discrimination test (with 130 ms pure tones of slightly different frequencies). In Studies 1 and 3 identification slope and phoneme boundary differences between reader groups were found as well. Though these results do not definitely prove the auditory perception hypothesis, they support it. The conditions that should be satisfied by auditory perception tasks in future reading acquisition research are briefly discussed.On various verbal-processing tasks, poorly reading children are less proficient than their normal or superior reading contemporaries (Vellutino, 1979). Several experiments suggest the core of poor readers' verbal-processing deficits concerns something about their use of phonological codes in short-term memory. The well-known "rhyming effect" studies (e.g.,
Three lO-step [paH~l continua were constructed with F2 as the single place-of-articulation cue. The continua were different in F3, which was rising, level, or falling. Identification tests using these continua were presented to 22 first-, second-, and fourth-grade children. Using logit analysis, individual phoneme boundary values were calculated for each F3 level. It was found that the phoneme-boundary shifts to slightly higher F2 starting values as age increases. Although no definite explanation is available yet, some possible interpretations are discussed.In De Weirdt (1985), age differences in a phoneme boundary were reported. First-grade children (tested at ages 6:2 and 6: 10), and third-and fourth-grade children (mean age 9:5) and adults took an identification test with three-formant stimuli on a The author is "aangesteld navorser" of the Belgian Nationaal Fonds voor Wetenschappelijk Onderzoek. Thanks are extended to M. Vanwonnhoudt, J. P. Martens, and B. Van Coile of tile Laboratory for Electronics and Metrology, for making available the equipment and some software for synthesizing thestimuli, and to G. De Soete and an anonymous reviewer for their helpful advice on an earlier draft of this paper. The author's mailing address is: Dienst voor Psychologie, Rijksuniversiteit Gent, H. Dunantlaan 2, B9000-Gent, Belgium.Maybe younger children are more influenced by the F3 cue-or less influenced by F2-than older ones. One might even speculate that speech categorization in younger children is generally based more on the higher frequency information in the acoustic signal. We were led to this presumption by the results of Irwin, Ball, Kay, Stillman, and Rosser (1985), who demonstrated that auditory temporal acuity develops differently in different frequency regions. They measured the minimum detectable gap in octave-band noises centered around 500, 1000, and 2000 Hz at various ages between 6 and 23 years. Especially between the ages of 6 and 10, the minimum detectable gap decreased, but did so substantially only with the 500-Hz stimulus. Irwin et al. (1985, seeFigure 2) did not cover the whole frequency range, but their data suggest that a general trend may exist. The difference between the level of temporal acuity at age 6 and that of the adult increases as one descends the frequency scale. This pattern might also apply to formants in speech sounds. Assuming that temporal processing aspects are probably crucial for the perceptual effectiveness of formant transition cues, the deviating [pa]-[b] phoneme boundaries of younger children may be due to poorer temporal resolution of the (lower frequency) F2 cue, resulting in higher "weights" for F3.It would be interesting to determine if the F3 cue is more important for younger children. If this were true, however, the developmental shift of the phoneme boundary would be only a stimulus artefact. Accordingly, the earlier study (De Weirdt, 1985) was replicated with stimulus continua at three F3-levels: falling, level, and rising. If younger subjects are more sensitive to t...
scite is a Brooklyn-based organization that helps researchers better discover and understand research articles through Smart Citations–citations that display the context of the citation and describe whether the article provides supporting or contrasting evidence. scite is used by students and researchers from around the world and is funded in part by the National Science Foundation and the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health.
customersupport@researchsolutions.com
10624 S. Eastern Ave., Ste. A-614
Henderson, NV 89052, USA
This site is protected by reCAPTCHA and the Google Privacy Policy and Terms of Service apply.
Copyright © 2025 scite LLC. All rights reserved.
Made with 💙 for researchers
Part of the Research Solutions Family.