1980
DOI: 10.1016/s0166-4115(08)61964-0
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

29 A Mental Clock Setting Process Reveated by Reaction Times

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2
1
1
1

Citation Types

2
14
1

Year Published

1982
1982
2014
2014

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

0
6

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 15 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 15 publications
2
14
1
Order By: Relevance
“…In this study, the RSIs speciWed the onset of the visual response cue and when to respond-information that could have been used for response scheduling. Our results are congruent with previous studies that demonstrated the use of a subtle temporal cue to minimize reaction time in discrete (Quesada & Schmidt, 1970;Rosenbaum & Patashnik, 1980) and continuous reaction time tasks (Willingham et al, 1997) in the laboratory as well as anticipatory scheduling in musical performance (ShaVer, 1981(ShaVer, , 1982(ShaVer, , 1984, sports (Lee et al, 1983), and pedestrian navigation (Rantanen & Xu, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…In this study, the RSIs speciWed the onset of the visual response cue and when to respond-information that could have been used for response scheduling. Our results are congruent with previous studies that demonstrated the use of a subtle temporal cue to minimize reaction time in discrete (Quesada & Schmidt, 1970;Rosenbaum & Patashnik, 1980) and continuous reaction time tasks (Willingham et al, 1997) in the laboratory as well as anticipatory scheduling in musical performance (ShaVer, 1981(ShaVer, , 1982(ShaVer, , 1984, sports (Lee et al, 1983), and pedestrian navigation (Rantanen & Xu, 2001).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 92%
“…A predictable temporal pattern might also allow for eYcient anticipatory scheduling of responses, leading to shorter reaction times. While it is known that people can use information about an expected time interval to minimize reaction time in discrete (Quesada & Schmidt, 1970;Rosenbaum & Patashnik, 1980) or continuous reaction time tasks (Willingham, Greenberg, & Thomas, 1997), whether implicitly learned temporal patterns could also be used for scheduling sequenced actions has not been explored systematically.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Many studies show more variability in interval duration for longer intervals defined by movement [84][85][86][87][88][89][90][91], and as explained in Schmidt et al [85, p. 422], these findings are expected in extrinsic timing models: 'the mechanism that meters out intervals of time . .…”
Section: (C) Evidence For Extrinsic Timingmentioning
confidence: 90%
“…Single-timed interval production, where participants reproduce a single interval to match the duration of a model, using, e.g. taps [86,87,90,94], for intervals ranging from 0 to 1050 ms. 2. Movements made to a metronome: for moving a stylus to and from a target, with interbeat intervals from 200 to 500 ms [85].…”
Section: (C) Evidence For Extrinsic Timingmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…(2) can be rewritten to solve for Var(N) as (6) All the terms in eq. (6) are constant except for Var(N) and E(I).…”
Section: An Alarm Clock Model·mentioning
confidence: 99%