2011
DOI: 10.1016/j.cogsys.2010.11.001
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A model for relational reasoning as verbal reasoning

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
20
0

Year Published

2012
2012
2019
2019

Publication Types

Select...
4
3
1

Relationship

1
7

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 25 publications
(22 citation statements)
references
References 41 publications
1
20
0
Order By: Relevance
“…Otherwise, entities have to be relocated within the preliminary model in order to regain consistency between this preliminary model and the additional information. The postulated single preliminary mental model that is varied when necessary, as opposed to the construction of multiple models, fits well with the principle of parsimony and with previous findings suggesting that humans prefer to construct as few mental models as possible (Knauff et al, 1995;Krumnack et al, 2011;Ragni et al, 2006;Ragni & Knauff, 2013;Ragni et al, 2005;Rauh et al, 2005;Rauh et al, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
See 1 more Smart Citation
“…Otherwise, entities have to be relocated within the preliminary model in order to regain consistency between this preliminary model and the additional information. The postulated single preliminary mental model that is varied when necessary, as opposed to the construction of multiple models, fits well with the principle of parsimony and with previous findings suggesting that humans prefer to construct as few mental models as possible (Knauff et al, 1995;Krumnack et al, 2011;Ragni et al, 2006;Ragni & Knauff, 2013;Ragni et al, 2005;Rauh et al, 2005;Rauh et al, 1997).…”
Section: Discussionsupporting
confidence: 84%
“…In other words, all objects are integrated into the model at the first free position that is not already occupied by another object and fulfils the spatial relation from the premise at hand (Knauff et al, 1995;Krumnack et al, 2011;Ragni et al, 2006;Ragni et al, 2005;Rauh et al, 2005;Rauh et al, 1997). Moreover, the preferences result from people's tendency to construct models from left to right (Chan & Bergen, 2005;De Soto et al, 1965;Hörnig et al, 2006;Huttenlocher, 1968;Spalek & Hammad, 2005) and to insert new entities to the endpoint on the rightmost side than to place new entities between already represented entities within a mental model (Krumnack et al, 2010(Krumnack et al, , 2011. Additionally, humans try to counteract the more difficult task to represent a greater number of entities in a mental model by chunking entities within a mental representation (Halford et al, 1998;Schaeken et al, 2007;Vandierendonck et al, 2004).…”
Section: Please Scroll Down For Articlementioning
confidence: 99%
“…Jahn, Knauff, & Johnson-Laird, 2007;Knauff & Ragni, 2011), and inferences that call for such rearrangements are more difficult than those that do not (e.g. Krumnack, Bucher, Nejasmic, Nebel, & Knauff, 2011). Analogous results bear out the use of iconic representations in temporal reasoning, whether it depends on relations such as "before" and "after" (Schaeken, Johnson-Laird, & d'Ydewalle, 1996a) or on the tense and aspect of verbs, as in:…”
Section: Icons and Symbolsmentioning
confidence: 80%
“…The systemic complexity of a system (also referred to as dynamic complexity) is a result of interactions of system components in such a way that the system produces outputs that are not easily predictable. Note that there are other definitions of complexity of a system, such as computational complexity (Krumnack et al, 2011), or Kolmogorov complexity (Li and Vitányi, 2009). Although these definitions make it easier to measure complexity, their usefulness in dynamic decision-making research is limited.…”
Section: Introductionmentioning
confidence: 99%