2015
DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.02.014
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A predictive coding framework for rapid neural dynamics during sentence-level language comprehension

Abstract: There is a growing literature investigating the relationship between oscillatory neural dynamics measured using electroencephalography (EEG) and/or magnetoencephalography (MEG), and sentence-level language comprehension. Recent proposals have suggested a strong link between predictive coding accounts of the hierarchical flow of information in the brain, and oscillatory neural dynamics in the beta and gamma frequency ranges. We propose that findings relating beta and gamma oscillations to sentence-level languag… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

29
201
5
1

Year Published

2015
2015
2023
2023

Publication Types

Select...
7
1

Relationship

0
8

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 201 publications
(236 citation statements)
references
References 90 publications
(140 reference statements)
29
201
5
1
Order By: Relevance
“…Finally, given the amount of emphasis on predictive mechanisms in current sentence processing research (e.g. Altmann & Mirkovic, 2009;Brothers, Swaab, & Traxler, 2015;Farmer, Brown, & Tanenhaus, 2013;Federmeier, 2007;Fine, Jaeger, Farmer, & Qian, 2013;Garrod & Pickering, 2015;Kim & Lai, 2012;Levy, 2008;Lewis & Bastiaansen, 2015;Van Petten & Luka, 2012; see Kuperberg & Jaeger, 2016 for a review), we believe that this method may be valuable in addressing a variety of research questions regarding how language users leverage predictive processes to help resolve conflicts online (e.g. in cases of co-reference resolution or syntactic ambiguity resolution).…”
Section: Methodological Contributionsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Finally, given the amount of emphasis on predictive mechanisms in current sentence processing research (e.g. Altmann & Mirkovic, 2009;Brothers, Swaab, & Traxler, 2015;Farmer, Brown, & Tanenhaus, 2013;Federmeier, 2007;Fine, Jaeger, Farmer, & Qian, 2013;Garrod & Pickering, 2015;Kim & Lai, 2012;Levy, 2008;Lewis & Bastiaansen, 2015;Van Petten & Luka, 2012; see Kuperberg & Jaeger, 2016 for a review), we believe that this method may be valuable in addressing a variety of research questions regarding how language users leverage predictive processes to help resolve conflicts online (e.g. in cases of co-reference resolution or syntactic ambiguity resolution).…”
Section: Methodological Contributionsmentioning
confidence: 98%
“…Unlike evoked ERP or MEG responses, which index phase-locked activity that is time-locked to specific events (Luck, 2014), and which are therefore best suited to detecting facilitation when a new incoming stimulus appears, low frequency oscillatory activity may be better suited for capturing top-down predictive neural activity (for general discussion, see Arnal & Giraud, 2012; Engel & Fries, 2010; Weiss & Mueller, 2012, and for recent discussion in relation to language comprehension, see Lewis & Bastiaansen, 2015). These studies generally used simple contexts that constrained strongly (versus weakly) for the perceptual features of new inputs.…”
Section: Section 3: Predictive Pre-activationmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Jordan & Rumelhart, 1992). Finally, it has been proposed that this type of hierarchical actively generative architecture is instantiated at the neural level in the form of predictive coding (Friston, 2005, 2008, 12 see Lewis & Bastiaansen, 2015 and Kuperberg, under review, for discussion in relation to the neural basis of language comprehension), although it is important to recognize that the most direct evidence for predictive coding in the brain comes from Rao and Ballard’s (1999) initial descriptions within the visual system. Given these considerations, we believe that this type of multi-representational hierarchical actively generative architecture can potentially provide a powerful bridge across the fields of computational linguistics, psycholinguistics and the neurobiology of language, and we hope that, by sketching out its principles, this will stimulate cross-disciplinary collaboration across these areas.…”
Section: Section 5: Towards a Hierarchical Multi-representational Genmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Despite the different frequency ranges of lower and middle gamma bands, and the different time development of the effects observed (sustained in time, Hald et al, 2006;Penolazzi et al, 2009;Wang et al, 2012;or short lasting, Monsalve et al, 2014), according to a recent interpretation of these changes the increase in gamma power would reflect reverberatory activity triggered by a successful match "between the pre-activation of the neural representation of the predicted word, and the neural representation of the actually incoming word" (Wang et al,p.11). This explanation (see also Lewis & Bastiaansen, 2015) processes involved in processing highly predictable strings. In idiomatic contexts, once the idiom is recognised, the processing of the remaining idiom constituents may be shallower if not absent and this led to the power differences on W2.…”
Section: Gamma Band Frequency and Semantic Compositionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In fact, power increase in lower and middle gamma frequency bands (approximately between 35-75 Hz) has been associated with successful semantic unification mechanisms (e.g., Hald, Bastiaansen & Hagoort, 2006;Penolazzi, Angrilli & Job, 2009;Wang, Zhu & Bastiaansen, 2012). Lewis and Bastiaansen (2015) proposed that such activity "reflects a match between strong top-down predictions and bottom-up linguistic input" (p.159). As we mentioned, Rommers et al (2013) directly contrasted the EEG activity in idiomatic and literal sentence processing in the time-frequency domain.…”
Section: Brain Electrical Correlates Of Semantic-pragmatic Integratiomentioning
confidence: 99%