2020
DOI: 10.1037/cep0000195
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A reexamination of consonant–vowel differences in masked transposed letter priming effects in the lexical decision task.

Abstract: Most orthographic coding models are based on the assumption that the orthographic code does not distinguish between vowels and consonants and, therefore, those models predict no difference between vowel (cisano-CASINO) and consonant (caniso-CASINO) transposed-letter (TL) effects. The available data, however, do provide some evidence for a consonant–vowel distinction at the level of the orthographic code. Most centrally, masked priming lexical decision tasks, mainly carried out in Spanish, have shown priming fr… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
2

Citation Types

0
3
0

Year Published

2021
2021
2022
2022

Publication Types

Select...
3

Relationship

1
2

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 3 publications
(3 citation statements)
references
References 67 publications
(193 reference statements)
0
3
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In adult readers, this might be expected to translate to more flexible orthographic codes for high frequency words relative to low frequency words. 3 Behavioral evidence for an interaction between frequency and the size of TL priming effects is elusive (e.g., Forster et al, 1987 ; Yang & Lupker, 2020 ). Note, however, that the interaction between language and TL priming also failed to reach significance in the language decision responses that we analyzed here.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In adult readers, this might be expected to translate to more flexible orthographic codes for high frequency words relative to low frequency words. 3 Behavioral evidence for an interaction between frequency and the size of TL priming effects is elusive (e.g., Forster et al, 1987 ; Yang & Lupker, 2020 ). Note, however, that the interaction between language and TL priming also failed to reach significance in the language decision responses that we analyzed here.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…Thus, perhaps when comparing priming effects of different sizes (or testing whether priming effects are present), a better manipulation would be to use conditions which are known to provide the maximal probability of producing an effect. Given that VV transpositions have been shown to give small or null priming effects in some situations (Lupker et al, 2008; Perea & Lupker, 2004; but see Yang & Lupker, 2020), it would be important to test the VV transposition effect with a condition in which priming effects are maximised (i.e., with adjacent transpositions). It might be interesting, therefore, to see whether our results would be replicated if adjacent transpositions were used in Spanish.…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…In the following years, similar patterns have been found in other masked priming lexical decision experiments in Spanish (Acha & Perea, 2008; Comesaña et al, 2016; Perea & Acha, 2009; see also Carreiras et al, 2007, 2009; Johnson, 2007) and in English (Lupker et al, 2008). More recently, however, Yang and Lupker (2020) were unable to replicate the pattern in English (i.e., consonant and vowel transpositions produced equivalent TL priming effects), a result that led those researchers to conclude that differences between consonants and vowels typically emerge at later stages than the orthographic coding stage, the stage that is typically examined with masked priming. Nonetheless, at least in Spanish, a language that is similar to Italian in many respects, there seems to be evidence for the existence of a consonant–vowel difference in TL priming effects.…”
mentioning
confidence: 99%