2016
DOI: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.09.007
|View full text |Cite
|
Sign up to set email alerts
|

A three-dimensional spatial characterization of the crossed-hands deficit

Abstract: To perceive the location of touch in space, we integrate information about skin-location with information about the location of that body part in space. Most research investigating this process of tactile spatial remapping has used the so-called crossed-hands deficit, in which the ability to judge the temporal order of touches on the two hands is impaired when the arms are crossed. This posture induces a conflict between skin-based and tactile external spatial representations, specifically in the left-right di… Show more

Help me understand this report

Search citation statements

Order By: Relevance

Paper Sections

Select...
1
1
1
1

Citation Types

1
16
0

Year Published

2017
2017
2020
2020

Publication Types

Select...
6

Relationship

1
5

Authors

Journals

citations
Cited by 19 publications
(17 citation statements)
references
References 34 publications
1
16
0
Order By: Relevance
“…In Figure , we compared performance of LM in the two postures with that of 343 participants from 12 published studies (Azañón et al., ; Azañón, Mihaljevic, et al., ; Azañón, Radulova, et al., ; Azañón & Soto‐Faraco, ; Badde et al., , ; Ley et al., ; Nishikawa et al., ; Roberts & Humphreys, ; Schicke & Röder, ; Wada et al., ; Yamamoto & Kitazawa, ) and two unpublished studies tested in similar bimanual crossed‐hand TOJ paradigms. LM's performance in the crossed‐hands posture was in the top 2–3% of the population (with a percentile of 98 and 97, for probit slopes and accuracy, respectively).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 3 more Smart Citations
“…In Figure , we compared performance of LM in the two postures with that of 343 participants from 12 published studies (Azañón et al., ; Azañón, Mihaljevic, et al., ; Azañón, Radulova, et al., ; Azañón & Soto‐Faraco, ; Badde et al., , ; Ley et al., ; Nishikawa et al., ; Roberts & Humphreys, ; Schicke & Röder, ; Wada et al., ; Yamamoto & Kitazawa, ) and two unpublished studies tested in similar bimanual crossed‐hand TOJ paradigms. LM's performance in the crossed‐hands posture was in the top 2–3% of the population (with a percentile of 98 and 97, for probit slopes and accuracy, respectively).…”
Section: Resultsmentioning
confidence: 99%
“…We investigated whether the use of an automatic remapping of touch into an external reference frame requires visual input during the first months of life. Extensive previous research has shown that normally sighted humans localize touch into external coordinates automatically (Yamamoto & Kitazawa, 2001;Azañ on, Camacho, & Soto-Faraco, 2010;Kitazawa, 2002). Here, we tested the spatial remapping of touch in a group of children who were born with cataracts and were, thus, deprived of vision for the first months of life (up to 5 months of age).…”
Section: Discussionmentioning
confidence: 99%
See 2 more Smart Citations
“…In other words, tactual attention can be directed to locations that are represented in a frame of reference that isn't somatotopic and, so, could potentially be used to also represent the locations of objects that are seen or heard. A similar conclusion can be extracted from experimental studies of the so-called "crossed-hands deficit", in which a subject's ability to judge the temporal order of tactile stimuli presented to her hands is impaired when her arms are crossed (Yamamoto and Kitazawa 2001;Shore and Spence 2002;Heed et al 2012;Azañón et al 2016).…”
Section: The Experience Of Egocentric Spacementioning
confidence: 68%